Skip to main content

Default mode network in young male adults with autism spectrum disorder: relationship with autism spectrum traits

Abstract

Background

Autism spectrum traits are postulated to lie on a continuum that extends between individuals with autism and individuals with typical development (TD). Social cognition properties that are deeply associated with autism spectrum traits have been linked to functional connectivity between regions within the brain’s default mode network (DMN). Previous studies have shown that the resting-state functional connectivities (rs-FCs) of DMN are low and show negative correlation with the level of autism spectrum traits in individuals with autism spectrum disorder (ASD). However, it is unclear whether individual differences of autism spectrum traits are associated with the strength of rs-FCs of DMN in participants including the general population.

Methods

Using the seed-based approach, we investigated the rs-FCs of DMN, particularly including the following two core regions of DMN: the anterior medial prefrontal cortex (aMPFC) and posterior cingulate cortex (PCC) in 19 young male adults with high-functioning ASD (mean age = 25.3 ± 6.9 years; autism-spectrum quotient (AQ) = 33.4 ± 4.2; full scale IQ (F-IQ) = 109.7 ± 12.4) compared with 21 age- and IQ-matched young male adults from the TD group (mean age = 24.8 ± 4.3 years; AQ = 18.6 ± 5.7; F-IQ = 109.5 ± 8.7). We also analyzed the correlation between the strength of rs-FCs and autism spectrum traits measured using AQ score.

Results

The strengths of rs-FCs from core regions of DMN were significantly lower in ASD participants than TD participants. Under multiple regression analysis, the strengths of rs-FCs in brain areas from aMPFC seed showed negative correlation with AQ scores in ASD participants and TD participants.

Conclusions

Our findings suggest that the strength of rs-FCs in DMN is associated with autism spectrum traits in the TD population as well as patients with ASD, supporting the continuum view. The rs-FCs of DMN may be useful biomarkers for the objective identification of autism spectrum traits, regardless of ASD diagnosis.

Background

Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is a complex neurodevelopmental disorder characterized by impaired social communication and social interaction, and unusually restricted, repetitive behaviors and interests [1, 2]. These characteristics are postulated to lie on a continuum that extends between individuals with autism and individuals with typical development (TD) [3, 4]. This continuum view suggests the possibility that autism spectrum traits are found not only at high levels in individuals with ASD but also at lower levels among individuals without ASD [57]. This continuum view shifts us away from merely categorical diagnosis towards the quantitative support of daily difficulties associated with autism spectrum traits of each individual regardless of ASD diagnosis. Taking the quantitative support into consideration necessitates the use of an instrument that can quantify autism spectrum traits in the entire population; such an instrument could also be used to define the broader autism phenotype [8, 9].

To identify the level of autism spectrum traits, several instruments have been developed. The autism-spectrum quotient (AQ) questionnaire is a useful instrument that was developed for identifying the extent of autistic spectrum traits, assuming that adults of normal intelligence may have autism spectrum traits at various levels [10]. Moreover, AQ is a validated measure of autism spectrum characteristics found within both the typical population and individuals with a diagnosis of autism [10, 11].

Recent behavioral studies have shown that individual differences in autism spectrum traits measured using AQ are associated with the performance of social cognition processing tasks such as self-focused attention, mind reading, episodic memory, and inferring others’ mental state in both a population with ASD and that with TD [1215]. Neuroimaging studies also demonstrate that autism spectrum traits measured using AQ are associated with structural and functional abnormalities in brain regions including the insula, inferior frontal gyrus, and posterior cingulate cortex (PCC), which are involved in social cognition processing in individuals with and without ASD [1618]. Taken together, these findings indicate that it is very important to evaluate the neuroimaging studies considering the level of autism spectrum traits.

Social and self-referential cognitive processes have been linked with cortical midline brain regions such as PCC and the medial prefrontal cortex (MPFC), reflecting the high functional connectivities within the default mode network (DMN) [19, 20]. In healthy individuals with TD, the functional connectivities of DMN are consistently deactivated when the individual is engaged in a cognitively demanding, goal-oriented task [21]. On the other hand, brain regions of the DMN are often engaged during social cognition processing tasks such as self-reference and the ‘theory of mind’ [2224]. This marked overlap between brain regions of the DMN and regions of the ‘social brain’ suggests that the brain regions of the DMN are strongly associated with the social cognition process [25, 26].

Several studies of the DMN in individuals with ASD have shown its lower functional connectivities in resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging (rs-fMRI), using the approaches of both region of interest (ROI) analysis and independent component analysis (ICA), than in individuals with TD [2732]. In addition, these studies also showed negative correlations between the strength of the resting-state functional connectivity (rs-FC) of DMN and autism spectrum traits including social deficits in individuals with ASD. However, these studies have primarily investigated correlations between the strength of rs-FCs of DMN and autism spectrum traits only in individuals with ASD [27, 29, 31]. To the best of our knowledge, the individual differences in autism spectrum traits regarding social cognition processing, which may also be associated with the strength of rs-FCs of DMN in individuals with or without ASD, have not been evaluated.

The aims of this study were: (1) to clarify the rs-FCs of DMN in high-functioning young male adults with ASD by comparison with age- and IQ-matched young male adults with TD on the basis of the location of ROIs within DMN during rs-fMRI; and (2) to evaluate correlations between the strength of rs-FCs of DMN and autism spectrum traits measured using AQ scores in TD participants and ASD participants, respectively.

Methods

Participants

Nineteen male individuals with high-functioning ASD were recruited by the Department of Neuropsychiatry at the University of Fukui Hospital, Japan, and the Department of Psychiatry and Neurobiology of the Kanazawa University Hospital, Japan. The authors (HK and TMu) diagnosed the participants on the basis of the criteria in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-IV-TR) [33] and standardized criteria taken from the Diagnostic Interview for Social and Communication Disorders (DISCO) [34]. The DISCO is reported to have good psychometric properties [35]. It also contains items on early development, and a section on activities of daily life, thereby giving the interviewer an idea of the individual’s level of functioning in several different areas, not only social functioning and communication [35]. All participants were right-handed [36]. Twenty-one age-matched and intelligence quotient (IQ)-matched TD male participants were recruited from the local community. Individuals with a history of major medical or neurological illness, including epilepsy, significant head trauma, or a lifetime history of alcohol or drug dependence, were excluded from this study. They were screened to exclude individuals who had a first degree relative with an Axis I disorder, diagnosed on the basis of the DSM-IV criteria. IQ was assessed using the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-III (WAIS-III) [37]. All the participants had full scale IQ (F-IQ) scores >85. There were no significant differences between the groups in terms of age, verbal IQ, performance IQ, and F-IQ (all P >0.5). All the participants also completed the AQ questionnaire [10]. The protocol used for this study was approved by the ethics committee of the University of Fukui. After a complete explanation of the study, all the participants provided written, informed consent. Their mean age, handedness, IQ and AQ score are shown in Table 1.

Table 1 Demographic data, IQ and AQ scores of participants

fMRI data acquisition

Functional images were acquired with T2*-weighted gradient-echo echo-planar imaging (EPI) sequence using a 3-T imager (Discovery MR 750; General Electric Medical Systems, Milwaukee, WI, USA) and a 32-channnel head coil. Two hundred and one volumes were acquired in each participant. Each volume consisted of 40 slices, with a thickness of 3.5 mm and a 0.5 mm gap to cover the entire brain. The time interval between two successive acquisitions of the same slice (repetition time, TR) was 2,300 ms, with an echo time (TE) of 30 ms and a flip angle (FA) of 81 degrees. The field of view (FOV) was 192 × 192 mm and the matrix size was 64 × 64, giving volume dimensions of 3 × 3 mm. The participants were instructed to close their eyes but stay awake and think of nothing in particular. A total of 201 volumes were acquired for a total imaging time of 7 min 42 s. The experiment was conducted at the Biomedical Imaging Research Center of the University of Fukui.

fMRI data analysis

Preprocessing

Data were analyzed using SPM8 software (Wellcome Department of Imaging Neuroscience, London, UK). After discarding the first five volumes, all volumes were realigned spatially to the mean volume, and the signal from each slice was realigned temporally to that obtained from the middle slice using sinc interpolation. The resliced volumes were normalized to the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) space with a voxel size of 2 × 2 × 2 mm using the EPI template of SPM8. The normalized images were spatially smoothed with a 6-mm Gaussian kernel.

Rs-fMRI datasets were processed using a toolkit of the Data Processing Assistant for Resting-State fMRI (DPARSF; http://www.restfmri.net) [38]. We conducted additional processing as follows: (1) removing the linear trend in the time series; and (2) performing temporally bandpass filtering (0.01-0.08 Hz) to reduce the effects of low-frequency drift and high-frequency noise [39, 40]. To control the non-neural noise in the time series [41]; (3) several sources of spurious variance, that is, six parameters from the rigid body correction of head motion, white matter signals, CSF signals, and global signals were removed from the data through linear regression [42].

Head movement parameters

Rs-FCs of DMN are significantly affected by the head motion of participants during fMRI scanning; that is, long-distance correlations are decreased by participant motion, whereas many short-distance correlations are increased [4347]. To investigate the effect of head motion and motion artifacts in rs-FCs, the root mean square (RMS) of six movement parameters obtained in the realignment process (x-, y-, z translations and x-, y-, z rotations), mean frame-to-frame RMS motion [43] and frame-wise displacement (FD) [45] were calculated for each participant. There were no significant differences in RMS (P values ranged from 0.17 to 0.70), mean frame-to-frame RMS motion (P value was 0.11) and FD (P value was 0.14) between the groups. In addition, there is no significant relationship between AQ scores and the six RMS head movement parameters (P values ranged from 0.10 to 0.78).

Definition of ROIs

To clarify the rs-FCs of DMN in the present study, we defined the regions in the anterior MPFC (aMPFC) and PCC as ROIs. The ROI coordinates were selected from the DMN meta-analysis [48]. The seed regions of the aMPFC and PCC comprise core seeds within the functional connectivity of DMN, and their widespread connectivities are supported by connectional anatomy studies [4850]. Two spherical ROIs of 8 mm radius centered at a coordinate listed in Table 2 were drawn for each participant in line.

Table 2 Significant differences in rs-FCs between groups with ASD and TD (TD >ASD)

Rs-FC analysis

After the processing of rs-fMRI data, we used the predefined seed regions for voxel-wise rs-FC analyses using the DPARSF toolbox. The mean time course of all voxels in each seed ROI was used to calculate voxel-wise linear correlations (Pearson’s correlation) in the whole brain during the rs-fMRI period. Individuals’ r values were normalized to z values using Fisher’s z transformation. At the group level, each image pertaining to z values reflecting a correlation between the seed ROI and each voxel of the brain was entered into a random effect two-sample t-test to identify the whole-brain regions showing significant differences between the groups with ASD and TD. In seed-based rs-FC analyses, global time series, computed across all brain voxels, along with six motion parameters, were used as additional covariates to remove confounding effects of physiologic noise and participant movement. For the presentation purpose, rs-FCs from the aMPFC and PCC seed ROIs are shown separately for each group at the threshold of P = 0.05 with a family wise error correction (Figure 1). Results were corrected between group comparisons, set at P = 0.001, uncorrected at peak level, and P = 0.05 with cluster level.

Figure 1
figure 1

Results of rs-FCs from the aMPFC or PCC seed for each group. (A) Rs-FCs from the aMPFC seed ROI are shown for each group. (B) Rs-FCs from the PCC seed ROI are shown for each group. The statistical threshold for both results was set at P = 0.05 with FWE correction and k = 10 voxels for presentation purpose. The region with functional connectivities was smaller in the ASD group than in the TD group. The coordinates (A) (-3, -60, 32); (B) (-4, -65, 24) at the figure) were selected from a previous study [52]. Color bar denotes t-statistic range.

Relationships among rs-FCs of group differences with AQ or IQ

To confirm the relationship between the significant difference in strength of rs-FCs and autism spectrum traits or IQ, we calculated correlations between AQ scores, F-IQ and the Z values, which were extracted from the regions showing the strength of rs-FCs of significant group differences (Spearman’s rank order correlation coefficients; the statistical threshold was set at P = 0.01).

Multiple regression analysis among the strength of rs-FC with AQ, IQ, or age

To investigate the relationships among the strength of rs-FC, autism spectrum traits, IQ, or age across each group, we performed additional analyses of nine independent variables in a multiple regression analysis in SPM8, AQ total scores, F-IQ, age, and RMS of six movement parameters as covariates. The statistical threshold for contrasts was P <0.001 uncorrected for height and cluster P <0.05 corrected for multiple comparisons. In addition, we tested whether the five AQ subscale scores would also show correlations with the brain regions and we observed correlations in each group.

Results

Differences in the strength of rs-FCs in DMN between groups

Rs-FCs from the aMPFC and PCC seed regions were distributed in the medial parts of the brain (Figure 1). In the ASD group compared with the TD group, substantially smaller areas were functionally connected with these seed regions.

In the ROIs within the DMN, significant differences in the strength of rs-FCs from the aMPFC seed were observed in the paracentral lobuli and middle frontal gyrus (MFG) between the two groups (Table 2, Figure 2). A significant difference in rs-FCs from the PCC seed was observed in the MPFC between the groups (Table 2, Figure 3). The mean correlation coefficient (Fisher z-transformed) for each group is shown in Figure 2 and Figure 3. The strengths of all of these rs-FCs were lower in the ASD group compared with the TD group. Individuals with ASD showed no significantly higher strength of rs-FCs from the seed regions compared with the TD group.

Figure 2
figure 2

Results of rs-FCs from the aMPFC seed ROI between groups. (A) Comparison of rs-FCs from the aMPFC seed ROI between groups. There were significant clusters in the paracentral lobuli (left) and middle frontal gyrus (right). Detailed information on these clusters is shown in Table 2. (B) Mean and one standard error of Fisher’s z-transformed correlation coefficients extracted from the left paracentral lobule (left), right paracentral lobule (middle), and middle frontal gyrus (right) clusters are shown for each group. Rs-FCs were lower in the ASD group than in the TD group. (C) Scatter plots showing the correlations of the strength of rs-FCs in the aMPFC with left paracentral lobule (left), right paracentral lobule (middle), and MFG (right) with AQ total scores in each group. (D) Scatter plots showing the correlations of the strength of rs-FC in the aMPFC with left paracentral lobule (left), right paracentral lobule (middle), and MFG (right) with full-scale IQ score in each group. The red bar and squares represent individuals with ASD, and the blue bar and circles represent participants with TD. Color bar denotes t-statistic range.

Figure 3
figure 3

Results of rs-FCs from the PCC seed ROI between groups. (A) Comparison of rs-FCs from the PCC seed ROI between groups. There was a significant difference in the medial prefrontal cortex. Detailed information on the cluster is shown in Table 2. (B) Mean and one standard error of Fisher’s z-transformed correlation coefficients extracted from the medial prefrontal cortex are shown for each group. Rs-FCs were lower in the ASD group than in the TD group. (C) Scatter plots showing the correlations of the strength of rs-FC in the PCC with MPFC with AQ total scores in each group. (D) Scatter plots showing the correlations of the strength of rs-FCs in the PCC with MPFC with full-scale IQ score in each group. The red bar and squares represent individuals with ASD, and the blue bar and circles represent participants with TD.

Relationship between the strength of rs-FCs of group differences and AQ or IQ

In the ASD group, there were no significant correlations between either the strength of rs-FCs and F-IQ or AQ total score (Figure 2; Figure 3, P >0.01). In the TD group, there were also no significant correlations between either the strength of rs-FCs and F-IQ or AQ total score (Figure 2; Figure 3, P >0.01).

Relationship with AQ or IQ in the multiple regression analysis

In the ASD group, AQ total scores were significantly negatively correlated with the strength of rs-FC in aMPFC with MFG and cerebellum (Table 3; Figure 4, P <0.01). We also found that two AQ subscale scores (communication scores and attention switching scores) also significantly negatively correlated with the strength of rs-FCs in aMPFC with MFG and cerebellum (P <0.05). In the TD group, AQ total scores were significantly negatively correlated with the strength of rs-FC in aMPFC with superior temporal gyrus (STG) and MTG (Table 3; Figure 4, P <0.01). We also found that three AQ subscale scores (social skill scores, communication scores, and attention switching scores) also significantly negatively correlated with the strength of rs-FCs in aMPFC with STG and MTG (P <0.01).

Table 3 Brain regions showing negative correlations between AQ and the strength of rs-FCs in multiple regression analysis
Figure 4
figure 4

Brain regions showing negative correlations between AQ and the strength of rs-FCs from aMPFC seed in each group. Brain regions showing negative correlations between AQ and strength of rs-FCs from aMPFC seed in multiple regression analysis. The statistical threshold for contrasts was P <0.001 uncorrected for height and cluster P <0.05 corrected for multiple comparisons. The scatterplots show the association of AQ and the strength of rs- FCs in selected regions. aMPFC, anterior medial prefrontal cortex; MFG, middle frontal gyrus; MTG, middle temporal gyrus; STG, superior temporal gyrus; rs-FC, resting state functional connectivity.

There were no significant correlations between the strength of rs-FCs and each participant’s F-IQ in all the participants, in the ASD group or in the TD group, in the multiple regression analysis (P >0.01).

Relationship with age in the multiple regression analysis

In the TD group, age was significantly positively correlated with the strength of rs-FCs in the aMPFC with MTG and PCC with cingulate gyrus (Table 4; Figure 5). We found no significant correlations between age and the strength of rs-FCs of DMN, in the ASD group or all the participants.

Table 4 Brain regions showing correlations between age and the strength of rs-FCs in multiple regression analysis
Figure 5
figure 5

Brain regions showing positive correlations between age and the strength of rs-FCs from seed regions in TD participants. Brain regions showing positive correlations between age and the strength of rs-FCs from seed regions in multiple regression analysis. The statistical threshold for contrasts was P <0.001 uncorrected for height and cluster P <0.05 corrected for multiple comparisons. The scatterplots show the association of age and the strength of rs-FCs in selected regions. aMPFC, anterior medial prefrontal cortex; CingG, cingulate gyrus; MTG, middle temporal gyrus; PCC, posterior cingulate cortex; rs-FC, resting state functional connectivity.

Discussion

We found that young male adults with high-functioning ASD display lower rs-FCs between two seeds (aMPFC and PCC) of DMN and other brain regions In addition, we found that autism spectrum traits measured using the AQ score are associated with the strength of rs-FCs of DMN that contain brain regions relevant to social cognition processing, in each TD participants and ASD participants, respectively. These results complement previous studies [26, 29, 31], suggesting that the strengths of rs-FCs of DMN are associated with autistic spectrum traits of social cognition processing in ASD participants. In addition, our results showed that this association between the strength of rs-FCs of DMN and autistic spectrum traits also exists not only in ASD participants but also in TD participants. These findings suggest that the strength of rs-FCs of DMN may underlie some of the autism spectrum traits, regardless of ASD diagnosis.

Difference in functional connectivities of DMN between ASD and TD groups

Our results mostly agree with those of other studies of rs-FCs of DMN [2732] that demonstrated lower rs-FCs of DMN in ASD. However, as for MPFC, the regions showing lower functional connectivities in previous studies are not always in agreement with those found in this study [2730, 32]. Differences in approach (seed-based or ICA) might have made direct comparisons between results somewhat challenging [51]. Andrews-Hanna et al.[49] demonstrated that the aMPFC of DMN is associated with social cognition processes including judgments or remembering trait adjectives about themselves compared with other people, whereas the dorsal medial prefrontal cortex (dMPFC) of DMN is associated with social cognition processes including self-referential judgments about their present situation or mental states. These findings taken together with our findings suggest that rs-FCs in brain regions including both aMPFC and dMPFC are associated with autism spectrum traits associated with processes of basic social cognition of self and others in individuals with ASD.

DMN associated with autism spectrum traits

In recent studies of rs-FCs of DMN in ASD, correlations have been investigated between autism spectrum traits and functional connectivities within brain areas such as the precuneus [27], MPFC [27, 29, 32], anterior cingulate cortex [27], and superior frontal gyrus [31]. However, most of these studies analyzed these correlations in only ASD participants. In general, these studies suggest negative correlation between the strength of rs-FCs of DMN and autism spectrum traits in individuals with ASD.

In the current findings, the strength of rs-FCs in brain areas from aMPFC seed showed negative correlations with AQ total score not only in ASD participants but also in TD participants in the multiple regression analysis, although these areas from aMPFC seed that showed negative correlations with AQ total scores in two groups were different from each other (Table 3). Moreover, among AQ subscale scores, communication scores and attention switching scores were related to the strength of rs-FCs of DMN in the ASD group. On the other hand, social skill scores, communication scores, and attention switching scores were related to the strength of rs-FCs of DMN in the TD group. Our findings suggest that the strength of rs-FCs of DMN might underlie the level of autism spectrum traits in participants without ASD diagnosis or with subthreshold autism spectrum traits, supporting the continuum view. In addition, considering the results of the multiple regression analysis using AQ subscale scores, the nature of autism spectrum traits, which affects the strength of rs-FCs of DMN in aMPFC seed, might be different in each group and the function of DMN might not always be the same between ASD and TD groups.

Age-related changes in functional connectivities of DMN

Particularly interesting from our viewpoint is the lack of consensus regarding the strength of rs-FC aspects of age variation with ASD. In relation to age-related changes in DMN in ASD, few studies have examined age correlates of functional connectivities by rs-fMRI. To the best of our knowledge, only one rs-fMRI study has examined the functional connectivities in children with ASD compared with age- and IQ-matched children with TD [52]. The previous study showed higher rs-FCs of DMN in PCC seeds in children with ASD than in children with TD [52], but we found that lower rs-FCs of DMN in PCC seeds in the ASD group and a relationship between rs-FCs of DMN in PCC seeds and age in the TD group. We speculate that the age-related changes in the functional connectivities of DMN in ASD and TD may be linked to differences in neurodevelopmental mechanisms in childhood and numerous variables including point of development. Future studies are required to explore the variation of rs-FCs of DMN with age for individuals with ASD.

Future directions

First, the size of each group in this study may be relatively too small to demonstrate a relationship between the various levels of autism spectrum traits and the strength of rs-FCs of DMN. Second, we defined the seed regions for rs-FC analyses on the basis of the location of ROIs within the DMN in accordance with a previous study. Although the seed-based analysis in the present research makes it possible to determine rs-FC precisely, this analysis did not reveal information about intrinsically connected networks and their interactions [51]. Future study is necessary to replicate these findings, using the ICA approach for exploring information about intrinsically connected networks. Third, we included global signal regression in generating the FC map. However, it is a quite controversial issue whether global signal regression changes resting-state correlations and produces negative correlations [53]. Thus, the findings used in the present study are not definitive, and future study is necessary to compare the results with confound regression strategies. Finally, our participants were only young male adults with high-functioning ASD. Future study is needed to clarify rs-FCs in both males and females, children with ASD, and ASD individuals without high-functioning. These additional researches will help provide more complete pictures that may clarify the etiology of ASD.

Conclusions

Young male adults with high-functioning ASD showed lower rs-FCs of DMN compared with age- and IQ-matched young male adults with TD. Moreover, the strength of rs-FCs of DMN was associated with autism spectrum traits in each ASD and TD group, regardless of ASD diagnosis. We propose that the strength of rs-FCs of DMN might underlie the level of autism spectrum traits and might be one of the potential biomarkers for the objective identification of the level of autism spectrum traits, regardless of ASD diagnosis.

Abbreviations

AMPFC:

Anterior medial prefrontal cortex

AQ:

Autism-spectrum quotient

ASD:

Autism spectrum disorder

DMN:

Default mode network

DMPFC:

Dorsal medial prefrontal cortex

IQ:

Intelligence quotient

PCC:

Posterior cingulate cortex

Rs-FC:

Resting-state functional connectivities

TD:

Typical development.

References

  1. Lai M-C, Lombardo MV, Chakrabarti B, Baron-Cohen S: Subgrouping the autism “spectrum”: reflections on DSM-5. PLoS Biol. 2013, 11: e1001544-10.1371/journal.pbio.1001544.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Autism and Developmental Disabilities Monitoring Network Surveillance Year 2008 Principal Investigators; Centers for Disease Control and Prevention: Prevalence of autism spectrum disorders -- Autism and Developmental Disabilities Monitoring Network, 14 Sites, United States, 2008. MMWR Surveill Summ. 2012, 61: 1-19.

    Google Scholar 

  3. Frith U: Autism and Asperger Syndrome. 1991, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press

    Book  Google Scholar 

  4. Baron-Cohen S: Mindblindness: An Essay on Autism and Theory of Mind. 1997, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press

    Google Scholar 

  5. Constantino JN, Hudziak JJ, Todd RD: Deficits in reciprocal social behavior in male twins: evidence for a genetically independent domain of psychopathology. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2003, 42: 458-467. 10.1097/01.CHI.0000046811.95464.21.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Skuse DH, Mandy W, Steer C, Miller LL, Goodman R, Lawrence K, Emond A, Golding J: Social communication competence and functional adaptation in a general population of children: preliminary evidence for sex-by-verbal IQ differential risk. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2009, 48: 128-137. 10.1097/CHI.0b013e31819176b8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Bölte S, Westerwald E, Holtmann M, Freitag C, Poustka F: Autistic traits and autism spectrum disorders: The clinical validity of two measures presuming a continuum of social communication skills. J Autism Dev Disord. 2011, 41: 66-72. 10.1007/s10803-010-1024-9.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Wheelwright S, Auyeung B, Allison C, Baron-Cohen S: Defining the broader, medium and narrow autism phenotype among parents using the Autism Spectrum Quotient (AQ). Mol Autism. 2010, 1: 10-10.1186/2040-2392-1-10.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Happé F, Ronald A, Plomin R: Time to give up on a single explanation for autism. Nat Neurosci. 2006, 9: 1218-1220. 10.1038/nn1770.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S, Skinner R, Martin J, Clubley E: The autism-spectrum quotient (AQ): Evidence from asperger syndrome/high-functioning autism, males and females, scientists and mathematicians. J Autism Dev Disord. 2001, 31: 5-17. 10.1023/A:1005653411471.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Woodbury-Smith MR, Robinson J, Wheelwright S, Baron-Cohen S: Screening Adults for Asperger Syndrome Using the AQ: A Preliminary Study of its Diagnostic Validity in Clinical Practice. J Autism Dev Disord. 2005, 35: 331-335. 10.1007/s10803-005-3300-7.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Lombardo MV, Barnes JL, Wheelwright SJ, Baron-Cohen S: Self-referential cognition and empathy in autism. PLoS One. 2007, 2: e883-10.1371/journal.pone.0000883.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Golan O, Baron-Cohen S, Hill JJ, Rutherford MD: The “reading the mind in the voice” test-revised: A study of complex emotion recognition in adults with and without autism spectrum conditions. J Autism Dev Disord. 2007, 37: 1096-1106. 10.1007/s10803-006-0252-5.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Komeda H, Kosaka H, Saito DN, Inohara K, Munesue T, Ishitobi M, Sato M, Okazawa H: Episodic memory retrieval for story characters in high-functioning autism. Mol Autism. 2013, 4: 20-10.1186/2040-2392-4-20.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S, Hill J, Raste Y, Plumb I: The “ Reading the Mind in the Eyes ” Test Revised Version: A Study with Normal Adults, and Adults with Asperger Syndrome or High-functioning Autism. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2001, 42: 241-251. 10.1111/1469-7610.00715.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Kosaka H, Omori M, Munesue T, Ishitobi M, Matsumura Y, Takahashi T, Narita K, Murata T, Saito DN, Uchiyama H: Smaller insula and inferior frontal volumes in young adults with pervasive developmental disorders. Neuroimage. 2010, 50: 1357-1363. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.01.085.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Ishitobi M, Kosaka H, Omori M, Matsumura Y, Munesue T, Mizukami K, Shimoyama T, Murata T, Sadato N, Okazawa H: Differential amygdala response to lower face in patients with autistic spectrum disorders: An fMRI study. Res Autism Spectr Disord. 2011, 5: 910-919. 10.1016/j.rasd.2010.10.005.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Morita T, Kosaka H, Saito DN, Ishitobi M, Munesue T, Itakura S, Omori M, Okazawa H, Wada Y, Sadato N: Emotional responses associated with self-face processing in individuals with autism spectrum disorders: An fMRI study. Soc Neurosci. 2012, 7: 223-239. 10.1080/17470919.2011.598945.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Greicius MD, Krasnow B, Reiss AL, Menon V: Functional connectivity in the resting brain: a network analysis of the default mode hypothesis. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2003, 100: 253-258. 10.1073/pnas.0135058100.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Raichle ME, MacLeod AM, Snyder AZ, Powers WJ, Gusnard DA, Shulman GL: A default mode of brain function. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2001, 98: 676-682. 10.1073/pnas.98.2.676.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Raichle ME, Snyder AZ: A default mode of brain function: A brief history of an evolving idea. Neuroimage. 2007, 37: 1083-1090. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.02.041.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Schilbach L, Eickhoff SB, Rotarska-Jagiela A, Fink GR, Vogeley K: Minds at rest? Social cognition as the default mode of cognizing and its putative relationship to the “default system” of the brain. Conscious Cogn. 2008, 17: 457-467. 10.1016/j.concog.2008.03.013.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Amodio DM, Frith CD: Meeting of minds: the medial frontal cortex and social cognition. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2006, 7: 268-277. 10.1038/nrn1884.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Gilbert SJ, Williamson IDM, Dumontheil I, Simons JS, Frith CD, Burgess PW: Distinct regions of medial rostral prefrontal cortex supporting social and nonsocial functions. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2007, 2: 217-226. 10.1093/scan/nsm014.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Buckner RL, Andrews‒Hanna JR, Schacter DL: The brain’s default network. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2008, 1124: 1-38. 10.1196/annals.1440.011.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Menon V: Large-scale brain networks and psychopathology: a unifying triple network model. Trends Cogn Sci. 2011, 15: 483-506. 10.1016/j.tics.2011.08.003.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Assaf M, Jagannathan K, Calhoun V: Abnormal functional connectivity of default mode sub-networks in autism spectrum disorder patients. Neuroimage. 2010, 53: 247-256. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.05.067.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Kennedy DP, Courchesne E: Functional abnormalities of the default network during self-and other-reflection in autism. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2008, 3: 177-190. 10.1093/scan/nsn011.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Monk C, Peltier S, Wiggins J, Weng S: Abnormalities of intrinsic functional connectivity in autism spectrum disorders. Neuroimage. 2009, 47: 764-772. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.04.069.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Kennedy DP, Redcay E, Courchesne E: Failing to deactivate: resting functional abnormalities in autism. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2006, 103: 8275-8280. 10.1073/pnas.0600674103.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Weng S, Wiggins J, Peltier S, Carrasco M: Alterations of resting state functional connectivity in the default network in adolescents with autism spectrum disorders. Brain Res. 2010, 1313: 202-214.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Von Dem Hagen EA H, Stoyanova RS, Baron-Cohen S, Calder AJ: Reduced functional connectivity within and between “social” resting state networks in autism spectrum conditions. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2013, 8: 694-701. 10.1093/scan/nss053.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Association AP: Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: DSM-IV-TR®. 2000, Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing

    Google Scholar 

  34. Nygren G, Hagberg B, Billstedt E, Skoglund A, Gillberg C, Johansson M: The Swedish version of the Diagnostic Interview for Social and Communication Disorders (DISCO-10). Psychometric properties J Autism Dev Disord. 2009, 39: 730-741. 10.1007/s10803-008-0678-z.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Wing L, Leekam SR, Libby SJ, Gould J, Larcombe M: The Diagnostic Interview for Social and Communication Disorders: background, inter-rater reliability and clinical use. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2002, 43: 307-325. 10.1111/1469-7610.00023.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Oldfield RC: The assessment and analysis of handedness: The Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia. 1971, 9: 97-113. 10.1016/0028-3932(71)90067-4.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Wechsler D: WAIS-III: Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale. 1997, San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation

    Google Scholar 

  38. Chao-Gan Y, Yu-Feng Z: DPARSF: A MATLAB Toolbox for “Pipeline” Data Analysis of Resting-State fMRI. Front Syst Neurosci. 2010, 4: 1-7.

    Google Scholar 

  39. Biswal B, Zerrin Yetkin F, Haughton VM, Hyde JS: Functional connectivity in the motor cortex of resting human brain using echo-planar mri. Magn Reson Med. 1995, 34: 537-541. 10.1002/mrm.1910340409.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Lowe MJ, Mock BJ, Sorenson JA: Functional Connectivity in Single and Multislice Echoplanar Imaging Using Resting-State Fluctuations. Neuroimage. 1998, 7: 119-132. 10.1006/nimg.1997.0315.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Fox MD, Snyder AZ, Vincent JL, Corbetta M, Van Essen DC, Raichle ME: The human brain is intrinsically organized into dynamic, anticorrelated functional networks. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2005, 102: 9673-9678. 10.1073/pnas.0504136102.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Satterthwaite TD, Elliott MA, Gerraty RT, Ruparel K, Loughead J, Calkins ME, Eickhoff SB, Hakonarson H, Gur RC, Gur RE, Wolf DH: An improved framework for confound regression and filtering for control of motion artifact in the preprocessing of resting-state functional connectivity data. Neuroimage. 2013, 64: 240-256.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Van Dijk KR A, Sabuncu MR, Buckner RL: The influence of head motion on intrinsic functional connectivity MRI. Neuroimage. 2012, 59: 431-438. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.07.044.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Müller R, Shih P, Keehn B: Underconnected, but how? A survey of functional connectivity MRI studies in autism spectrum disorders. Cereb Cortex. 2011, 21: 2233-2243. 10.1093/cercor/bhq296.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Power JD, Barnes KA, Snyder AZ, Schlaggar BL, Petersen SE: Spurious but systematic correlations in functional connectivity MRI networks arise from subject motion. Neuroimage. 2012, 59: 2142-2154. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.10.018.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Satterthwaite TD, Wolf DH, Loughead J, Ruparel K, Elliott MA, Hakonarson H, Gur RC, Gur RE: Impact of in-scanner head motion on multiple measures of functional connectivity: relevance for studies of neurodevelopment in youth. Neuroimage. 2012, 60: 623-632. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.12.063.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Deen B, Pelphrey K: Perspective: Brain scans need a rethink. Nature. 2012, 491: S20-10.1038/491S20a.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Buckner RL, Sepulcre J, Talukdar T, Krienen FM, Liu H, Hedden T, Andrews-Hanna JR, Sperling RA, Johnson KA: Cortical hubs revealed by intrinsic functional connectivity: mapping, assessment of stability, and relation to Alzheimer’s disease. J Neurosci. 2009, 29: 1860-1873. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5062-08.2009.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Andrews-Hanna JR, Reidler JS, Sepulcre J, Poulin R, Buckner RL: Functional-anatomic fractionation of the brain’s default network. Neuron. 2010, 65: 550-562. 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.02.005.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Hagmann P, Cammoun L, Gigandet X, Meuli R, Honey CJ, Wedeen VJ, Sporns O: Mapping the structural core of human cerebral cortex. PLoS Biol. 2008, 6: e159-10.1371/journal.pbio.0060159.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Joel SE, Caffo BS, van Zijl PCM, Pekar JJ: On the relationship between seed-based and ICA-based measures of functional connectivity. Magn Reson Med. 2011, 66: 644-657. 10.1002/mrm.22818.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Lynch CJ, Uddin LQ, Supekar K, Khouzam A, Phillips J, Menon V: Default mode network in childhood autism: posteromedial cortex heterogeneity and relationship with social deficits. Biol Psychiatry. 2013, 74: 212-219. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.12.013.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Murphy K, Birn RM, Handwerker DA, Jones TB, Bandettini PA: The impact of global signal regression on resting state correlations: Are anti-correlated networks introduced?. Neuroimage. 2009, 44: 893-905. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.09.036.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We would like to express our sincere appreciation to the participants and their families, who generously and courageously participated in this research. This research was funded in part by Grants-in-Aid for Scientific Research from the Japan Society for the Promotion of Science (21220005, 21591509, 21791120, 25293248), the Japan Research Foundation for Clinical Pharmacology, and the Takeda Science Foundation. Part of this research was the result of ‘Development of biomarker candidates for social behavior’ and ‘Integrated research on neuropsychiatric disorders’ performed under the Strategic Research Program for Brain Sciences by the MEXT of Japan.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Hirotaka Kosaka.

Additional information

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Authors’ contributions

MJ was involved in conducting the experiment, analyzing and interpreting data, and drafting the article. HK was involved in recruiting the participants, diagnosing the participants with ASD, conducting the experiment, analyzing and interpreting data, and drafting the article. DNS, TM (fifth author), and KI were involved in recruiting the participants and conducting the experiment. MI was involved in recruiting the participants, interpreting data, and drafting the article. TM (eighth author) was involved in recruiting the participants and diagnosing the participants with ASD. MA, SA, AT, YW, NS, and HO were involved in interpreting the data. TI was involved in designing, analyzing and interpreting data, and drafting the article. All the authors have read and approved the final manuscript.

Authors’ original submitted files for images

Rights and permissions

Open Access  This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made.

The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder.

To view a copy of this licence, visit https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.

The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (https://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Jung, M., Kosaka, H., Saito, D.N. et al. Default mode network in young male adults with autism spectrum disorder: relationship with autism spectrum traits. Molecular Autism 5, 35 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1186/2040-2392-5-35

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/2040-2392-5-35

Keywords