Skip to main content

Self-referential and social cognition in a case of autism and agenesis of the corpus callosum

Abstract

Background

While models of autism spectrum conditions (ASC) are emerging at the genetic level of analysis, clear models at higher levels of analysis, such as neuroanatomy, are lacking. Here we examine agenesis of the corpus callosum (AgCC) as a model at the level of neuroanatomy that may be relevant for understanding self-referential and social-cognitive difficulties in ASC.

Methods

We examined performance on a wide array of tests in self-referential and social-cognitive domains in a patient with both AgCC and a diagnosis of ASC. Tests included a depth-of-processing memory paradigm with self-referential and social-cognitive manipulations, self-report measures of self-consciousness, alexithymia, and empathy, as well as performance measures of first-person pronoun usage and mentalizing ability. The performance of the AgCC patient was compared to a group of individuals with ASC but without AgCC and with neurotypical controls. These comparison groups come from a prior study where group differences were apparent across many measures. We used bootstrapping to assess whether the AgCC patient exhibited scores that were within or outside the 95% bias-corrected and accelerated bootstrap confidence intervals observed in both comparison groups.

Results

Within the depth-of-processing memory paradigm, the AgCC patient showed decreased memory sensitivity that was more extreme than both comparison groups across all conditions. The patient’s most pronounced difficulty on this task emerged in the social-cognitive domain related to information-processing about other people. The patient was similar to the ASC group in benefiting less from self-referential processing compared to the control group. Across a variety of other self-referential (i.e. alexithymia, private self-consciousness) and social-cognitive measures (i.e. self-reported imaginative and perspective-taking subscales of empathy, mentalizing), the AgCC patient also showed more extreme scores than those observed for both of the comparison groups. However, the AgCC patient scored within the range observed in the comparison groups on measures of first-person pronoun usage and self-reported affective empathy subscales.

Conclusions

We conclude that AgCC co-occurring with a diagnosis of ASC may be a relevant model at the level of neuroanatomy for understanding mechanisms involved in self-referential and high-level social-cognitive difficulties in ASC.

Background

Autism is a neurodevelopmental condition characterized by marked impairments in the domains of social interaction, language/communication, and pronounced repetitive stereotyped behaviors and/or restricted interests. The etiology of autism is complex as many genetic syndromes characterized by single-gene mutations (e.g., fragile X syndrome, Rett syndrome, tuberous sclerosis, Timothy syndrome) can result in a phenotype that meets the diagnostic criteria of autism (i.e. ‘syndromic autism’) [1]. Furthermore, research on non-syndromic forms of autism show that many simplex cases (up to 10% to 20%, but possibly more) possess de novo mutations in segments of DNA (i.e. deletions and/or duplications) presented as copy number variations [24]. This has resulted in the idea that there may be many types of autism arising from a complex combination of many different mechanisms.

In order to decompose some of this complexity, there is a need for clear models at a variety of levels of analysis. Autism genetics has already provided several useful models for understanding how genetic mechanisms linked to autism may have pathophysiological impact [58]. However, we currently lack clear models at higher levels of analysis such as at the level of neuroanatomy. In this paper, we focus on agenesis of the corpus callosum as one model at the level of neuroanatomy for dissecting aspects of self-referential and social-cognitive difficulties in autism. More specifically, we focus on areas within the self-referential domain related to the cognitive benefits of self-referential information processing (i.e. the self-reference effect in memory) as well as difficulties in emotional awareness (i.e. alexithymia), which are known to be more pronounced in higher-functioning adults with ASC. Within the social-cognitive domain, we focus on higher-level social-cognitive understanding of others (i.e. mentalizing, empathy), as well as memory for social agents; each of which have been shown to be sensitive to deficits in higher-functioning adults with ASC.

Agenesis of the corpus callosum (AgCC) is a congenital condition manifested in the failure to completely develop a corpus callosum; the largest white matter tract connecting the two cerebral hemispheres [9, 10]. AgCC can present as a complete or partial absence of the CC. Complex AgCC occurs with the presence of other confounding brain abnormalities such as polymicrogyria, heterotopia, or schizencephaly, whereas isolated AgCC does not occur with such abnormalities. However, isolated AgCC commonly co-occurs with colpocephaly (i.e. dilation of the posterior section of the lateral ventricles and reduction of ipsilateral cortical association tracts) and Probst bundles [10]. The etiologies of AgCC are complex and likely reflect a multitude of routes important for fully establishing callosal connections (i.e. cellular proliferation and migration, axon guidance and growth, glial development, and midline patterning). Many genetic syndromes are associated with AgCC (e.g., X-linked lissencephaly, Mowat-Wilson syndrome, CRASH syndrome), and some of these syndromes have been associated with autism (i.e. ARX mutations leading to X-linked lissencephaly [11]). However, like autism, the majority of AgCC cases do not have an identifiable single genetic cause [10]. There are important clinical and neuroanatomical parallels between AgCC and autism that may suggest the role of atypical callosal development in understanding the emergence of phenotypes associated with autism.

First, one of the more replicable findings in neuroscience research on autism is marked atypicalities in the corpus callosum (CC). In a meta-analysis of volumetric MRI studies on the CC in autism, Frazier and Hardan [12] found that all subsections are consistently observed as reduced in volume. This result is further bolstered by longitudinal evidence for persistence of CC volumetric reduction at a 2-year follow-up examination [13]. In diffusion MRI work, several atypicalities in CC integrity, particularly with respect to reduced fractional anisotropy and increased mean diffusivity are reported [1420]. Finally, a recent study using magnetization transfer imaging as an index of processes relevant to myelination found evidence for atypical myelination of the CC in autism [21]. The mounting evidence for atypical CC development in autism, particularly with respect to reduced size, potentially indicates that mechanisms involved in AgCC may also be important in autism.

Second, there are important clinical aspects that overlap between AgCC and autism. One of the most well known cases of such overlap is the case of the real ‘Rainman’, Kim Peek, who was diagnosed with autism well before later MRI scans determined he also had complete AgCC. Approximately 3% to 5% of individuals with neurodevelopmental conditions also have AgCC or CC hypoplasia [22, 23], and this is of particular relevance for autism, where CC volume is notably reduced [12]. In addition, 8.5% of individuals with AgCC also have a specific diagnosis of autism [24]. However, this estimate may be conservative [9, 25]. Badaruddin et al. [26] reported that in a sample of 61 two- to eleven-year-old children with AgCC, as many as 34% met criteria for at least one item in the domain of social impairment, 25% met criteria for at least one item in the communicative domain (with 51% showing difficulty in sustaining conversation), and up to 28% had a preoccupation with a specific interest. More recently, in 106 patients with AgCC, Lau et al. [27] found that 43% of children, 35% of adolescents, and 18% of adults score above cut-offs on the Autism Spectrum Quotient (AQ) that are typically used for screening for clinical diagnoses of autism spectrum conditions (ASCs).

Among the most prominent findings in the neuropsychological profile of AgCC are impairments in self-referential and social cognition [10]. Within the social-cognitive domain, many atypical findings in AgCC overlap with those seen in autism. Theory of mind or mentalizing impairments have long been known in autism [28, 29], and these impairments can be helpful for explaining difficulties in pragmatic aspects of social communication [30]. Deficits in cognitive aspects of empathy [3133] and on self-report measures of empathy [3438] have been consistently reported in ASC. Affective components of empathy are spared in ASC [31, 33], although self-report measures of this component are somewhat inconsistent. Some studies show decreases on subscales such as empathic concern [35, 39] but increases on subscales such as personal distress [35]. The latter subscale is likely linked to comorbid anxiety traits in ASC [40]. Still other reports of self-reported affective empathy show no differences [36, 38]. Given difficulties in ASC with self-referential cognitive processing, it is likely that the experimental studies on the topic comparing ASC to other groups with dissociable empathy deficits, such as individuals with psychopathic tendencies or conduct disorder [31, 33], give the clearest indication of the empathy profile in ASC rather than relying solely on self-report measures.

Several studies now show that individuals with AgCC have marked impairments in pragmatic aspects of language processing, including comprehension of idioms, proverbs, vocal prosody, and non-literal interpretation of humorous statements [4144]. Outside of pragmatic social communication difficulties, AgCC also shares social-cognitive deficits with those seen in autism. These difficulties extend into difficulty providing narratives that demonstrate understanding of social-emotional aspects of stories [4547]. There are mixed reports regarding AgCC and ToM deficits. One study found deficits on the Reading the Mind in the Eyes test in some, but not all, AgCC patients [25]. However, other work on adolescents and adult patients did not reveal deficits on other advanced ToM stories and faux pas tests [48] known to be sensitive to deficits within autism [49, 50]. While Symington et al. [48] did not find such advanced ToM deficits, they found difficulties in AgCC when asked to interpret videotaped social vignettes which indicated subtle deficits in emotion recognition, understanding sarcasm, and difficulty interpreting textual cues [48]. Finally, AgCC patients tend to underestimate emotional valence and intensity from negatively valenced pictures [51]. However, no studies exist in AgCC that comprehensively test components of empathy, making comparison with ASC difficult.

Prior work in the domain of self-referential cognition also indicates that difficulties in autism [35, 5254] may be shared by patients with AgCC. First, individuals with ASC show deficits across a wide range of areas where self-relevant information processing is critical, such as emotional understanding (e.g., alexithymia) [55], memory [35, 56], introspection [57], pronoun usage [35, 58], orienting to name [59, 60], monitoring own intentions [61], remembering own false beliefs [62], attributing privileged access to self over close others [63] (for review see [52, 53, 56, 58, 64, 65]). Within AgCC, one case study highlighted the possibility that alexithymia may be more pronounced [66]. This observation is similar to other reports on alexithymia hypothesizing that one aspect of its neurophysiological basis is reduced interhemispheric transfer [6669]. Parental reports indicate that patients with AgCC show poor personal insight [10]. Also indicative of a potential lack of insight is the effect that parental ratings tend to be higher than self-reported ratings on the AQ [27]. Analysis of narrative content indicates that patients with AgCC tend to use more first-person pronouns than comparison groups [46]. Finally, Brown and Paul [70] suggest that high scores on the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory Lie scale (without elevations in other scales) may be indicative of poor self-awareness in the two AgCC cases they tested.

Despite the evidence reviewed here with regard to self-referential and social cognition, much work is still needed. At present, very few studies on AgCC exist (e.g., [48]) that assess social-cognitive abilities known to be clearly atypical in autism. In addition, even less research in AgCC specifically tests known self-referential atypicalities in autism. In this study, we add to this literature by using a battery that measures processes such as empathy, mentalizing, alexithymia, self-referential memory biases, private self-consciousness, and implicit self-focused attention. We also attempt to go further by assessing a case with both partial AgCC and autism in comparison to both neurotypical controls and cases of autism without AgCC. By comparing this single case to the two groups, we examined the extent to which aspects of his self-referential and social-cognitive profile overlap with the pattern found in autism and also identified aspects which may be even more atypical than those seen in autism. From the viewpoint that individuals with autism typically have smaller CCs than controls, AgCC could be construed as an extreme of this profile. Therefore, highlighting areas in AgCC that are even more extreme compared to the autism group may provide some indication that the CC is critical for such aspects of self-referential and social cognition.

Methods

Patient AG

The patient reported in this study has diagnoses of partial AgCC and Asperger syndrome (AS) and will be referred to from here on as patient AG. AG was a participant of a larger multi-centre MRI study of ASC: the MRC AIMS Consortium study [54, 7178]. AG gave informed consent for the study in accordance with ethics approval from the National Research Ethics Committee, Suffolk, England. Upon AG’s recruitment, we considered him as having only a clinical diagnosis of AS, but later his AgCC was incidentally found by neuroradiological examination of his brain structural MRI scans. In keeping with the MRC AIMS protocol, both the Autism Diagnostic Interview–Revised (ADI–R) and the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS) were conducted before MRI scanning to confirm clinical diagnosis of ASC. AG scored above the cut-offs on both the ADI–R and ADOS (see Table 1 for scores and demographic information). In accordance with the recruitment criteria for the MRC AIMS Consortium, AG did not have any other comorbid medical (e.g., epilepsy) or neuropsychiatric conditions (e.g., attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder, obsessive-compulsive disorder). AG’s level of adaptive functioning was not rigorously assessed with any standardized measures, but parental report indicated limited living independence (i.e. living with parents, no full-time employment).

Table 1 Participant characteristics a

After completing the MRI scan (see Figure 1), it was immediately apparent that AG lacked almost all of the CC. A partial segment of the CC was intact in the position of what would typically be the splenium and/or isthmus. However, given the variability in positioning and connectivity of partial segments of the CC in partial AgCC, we cannot say with certainty that the splenium/isthmus was the subsection that was intact [79]. The anterior commissure was visible and intact. However, Probst bundles were visibly emanating in an anterior direction from the intact section of the CC. There were also signs of colpocephaly (i.e. dilation of the posterior aspect of the lateral ventricles.) There were no signs of polymicrogyria or heterotopic sigmoid bundles connecting occipital and frontal cortex. An independent radiologist confirmed these observations and converged upon the same diagnosis. AG’s general practitioner was informed immediately in accordance with standard operating procedures for incidental MRI findings.

Figure 1
figure 1

Structural MRI scan of patient AG. AG has partial agenesis of the corpus callosum (AgCC) and a clinical diagnosis of an autism spectrum condition (ASC). AG also showed signs of colpocephaly; dilation of the posterior aspect of the lateral ventricles.

Comparison groups

In order to assess self-referential and social cognition in AG, we used the same battery of tests reported in our previous paper on autism [35]. In that paper, we found across a variety of different measures a robust pattern of co-existing deficits in autism within the domains of self-referential and social-cognitive processing. Because we wanted to compare AG’s presentation to these patterns in autism, the control and ASC groups from this previous study [35] were used to provide comparison data to AG. All control and ASC participants in the prior study gave informed consent in accordance with ethics approval from the University of Cambridge Psychology Research Ethics Committee. There were 23 males and 7 females ages 19 to 45 years who had a formal diagnosis based on DSM-IV or ICD-10 criteria of either high-functioning autism (HFA; n = 4) or AS (n = 26). Controls consisted of 23 males and 7 females who were pairwise-matched with the ASC participants on age and sex and had no known psychiatric, developmental, or neurological disorders. All participants completed the AQ [80], and the ASC group scored higher than controls (P < 0.001). All participants scored within the normal range on the Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI) [81], and there were no statistically significant group differences on verbal, performance, or full-scale IQ (all P > 0.90). See Table 1 for participant characteristics.

Procedure and measures

Self-reference effect in memory (SRE) paradigm

The main experimental paradigm for testing both self-referential and social-cognitive information processing was the self-reference effect in memory paradigm (SRE). This paradigm allows participants to encode trait adjectives by judging them in relation to how descriptive they are of themselves or other people. Other encoding manipulations in this paradigm might involve semantic encoding manipulations or judgments based on basic linguistic characteristics. After encoding, participants are given a surprise memory test, and the typical trend is for heightened memory sensitivity for information previously encoded in relation to oneself when compared to other encoding conditions (i.e. thinking about others, semantic manipulations, linguistic characteristics). There is generally a gradient in memory performance across this paradigm, with the best memory for self-relevant information, followed by memory for others, semantic manipulations, and worst memory for shallow encoding based on basic linguistic characteristics. The SRE paradigm is a ‘depth-of-processing’ paradigm [82] because it is thought that the ‘deepest’ level of encoding in memory tends to be around structures that can easily organize and elaborate on information and the self possesses such properties that allow for deep organization and elaboration of information [8385].

For the current study, in the encoding phase, participants judged trait adjectives in one of four ways. In the ‘self’ condition, participants judged how descriptive the adjective was of themselves. In the ‘similar close other’ condition, adjectives were judged on how descriptive they were of their best friend. In the ‘dissimilar non-close other’ condition, participants judged whether the adjective was descriptive of Harry Potter [86]. All of these judgments were made on a 6-point scale, where 1 indicated ‘not at all descriptive’ and 6 indicated ‘very descriptive’. Finally, in the non-social control condition, participants judged how many syllables each adjective contained (from 1 to 6).

Each condition had 30 trials, and all were presented in pseudorandom order. All adjectives were drawn from a previously validated and widely used set of trait adjectives [87]. Half the adjectives in each condition were positively valenced (e.g., inventive), and the other half were negatively valenced (e.g., messy). Among all conditions, there were no differences in the number of characters, syllables, valence, or frequency of the adjectives. After encoding, there was a 30-minute delay before the retrieval phase. Participants were completely unaware of the subsequent recognition memory task to follow. During this delay, participants completed the performance section of the WASI. These tasks were nonverbal and were administered to keep the participant occupied during the delay period.

After the delay, participants were given a surprise recognition memory test. All 120 adjectives from encoding and 120 new distracter adjectives were presented in pseudorandom order. Participants judged their confidence in whether the adjective was ‘old’ or ‘new’. Confidence judgments were made on a 1 to 6 scale, where 6 was ‘definitely OLD’ and 4 was ‘OLD, but kind of unsure’. Conversely, a 1 indicated that they were ‘definitely NEW’ and 3 was ‘NEW, but kind of unsure’. This 6-point scale was used to force participants to make finer-grained recognition judgments and also to investigate whether there were any differences in how each group used different confidence judgments. Because there were no group differences among judgments on each scale point within any of the conditions, we collapsed judgments 1 through 3 into ‘New” and 4 through 6 into ‘old’ judgments. Furthermore, because all of the data from the control and ASC comparison groups come from a previously published report, we refer to that paper for details on the manipulation checks to ensure that participants did perceive best friends as more similar and closer to self than Harry Potter (see Manipulation Check section [35]). Finally, the dependent variable from this paradigm was a standard measure of memory sensitivity (d′), formulated as the standardized score of correctly remembered words minus the standardized score of false alarms. Higher d′ scores indicate enhanced memory sensitivity.

Additional self-referential and social-cognitive measures

In addition to the SRE paradigm, we included three other measures in the domain of self-referential cognition. These measures are included as a supplement to the self-referential memory sensitivity measure from the SRE paradigm and because they are theoretically relevant measures for testing more generalized deficits in self-referential processing in ASC [35]. If self-referential processing is atypical, then we expect to observe atypical performance across these additional measures as well as on the self-condition from the SRE paradigm. Self-report measures such as the Private Self-Consciousness Scale (PSCS) [88] and the Toronto Alexithymia Scale (TAS) [89] were used to measure explicit self-focused attention and emotional awareness. The TAS was further split into difficulty identifying feelings (DIF), difficulty describing feelings (DDF), and externally oriented thinking (EOT) subscales. On the TAS and all its subscales, higher scores indicate more alexithymic traits and can be interpreted as more impaired. We also included a performance measure of implicit self-focused attention called the Self-Focus Sentence Completion task [90]. In this task, participants were given sentence stems that included a self-reference (e.g., “I think…,” or “If I had my way…”). For each stem, we asked participants to complete the sentence in whatever way they liked. We computed an index of implicit self-focused attention (SFA) by automatically computing the percentage of first-person pronouns used to complete the stems (e.g., I, me, myself) using the Linguistic Inquiry and Word Count program [91]. This index has been used previously as a quantitative index of SFA [9294]. Higher scores on this index indicate more implicit self-focused attention.

In addition to the two ‘other’ conditions within the SRE paradigm, we included further measures in the domain of ‘other-oriented’ social-cognitive processing. Again, similar to the rationale behind the additional self-referential measures, these measures were included as a supplement to the social-cognitive memory sensitivity measures observed on the SRE paradigm and because they are theoretically relevant measures for testing more generalized deficits in social-cognitive processing in ASC [35]. If social-cognitive processing is atypical, then we expect to observe atypical performance across these additional measures as well as on the Friend and Potter conditions of the SRE paradigm. Three self-report measures of empathy were used: the Empathy Quotient (EQ) [34], the Interpersonal Reactivity Index (IRI) [95], and the Emotional Contagion Scale (ECS) [96]. The IRI was further split into perspective-taking (IRI-PT), empathic concern (IRI-EC), fantasy (IRI-FS), and personal distress (IRI-PD) subscales. Finally, we included the ‘Reading the Mind in the Eyes’ test (RMET) as one advanced performance measure of mentalizing/theory of mind [32].

Statistical analyses

To estimate similarities and differences between AG and the control and ASC comparison groups, bootstrapping (1 million resamples) [97] was used to estimate bias-corrected and accelerated 95% confidence intervals around the mean statistic from the control and ASC groups. AG’s performance was then compared to these bootstrap distributions. If AG’s performance was outside the bootstrap confidence intervals, we inferred that AG’s performance was significantly more extreme than performance on average within the comparison group.

Because AG had IQ scores below the comparison groups, further correlation analyses were run on the ASC group only (the one comparison group that partially overlaps with AG’s clinical presentation of ASC) in order to examine whether IQ, particularly verbal IQ (VIQ), was related to any of the dependent variables. For these analyses, we used robust regression [98] to estimate correlations with IQ that are robust to the influence of outlying data points.

Results

Table 1 shows participant characteristics for patient AG and the comparison groups. AG was similar to the mean ages of the two comparison groups. On IQ scales, AG was approximately 2 standard deviations below the mean of the comparison groups. Given the IQ differences, for all further analyses, we ran robust regression within the ASC group to assess the correlation between VIQ and the dependent measures while being insensitive to outliers. Any significant correlations within the ASC group would signal potentially that AG’s performance might be influenced by lower IQ than the comparison groups.

On the SRE paradigm, AG’s performance was significantly worse than both the ASC and control groups on all conditions, as noted by performance below the lower-bound 95% bootstrap confidence interval. The conditions with the largest deficits in performance were in social-cognitive conditions (Friend and Potter), whereas more subtle deficits were apparent across Self and Syllable conditions (see Figure 2 and Table 2). These results could not easily be explained simply by generalized slowed speed of processing in AgCC [99], because our patient responded with speed similar to, or in some cases faster than, both comparison groups across encoding and retrieval phases (see Additional file 1: Figure S1). Correlations between VIQ and memory sensitivity were assessed next to ascertain if AG’s performance might be explained by correlation between IQ and memory sensitivity in the ASC group. Here we found that correlations were positive for all conditions and were nominally significant (i.e. P < 0.05 but did not survive corrections for multiple comparisons) for VIQ in relation to Friend (r = 0.51, P = 0.0134) and Potter (r = 0.45, P = 0.0268) conditions (see Additional file 1: Figure S2). This potentially suggests that worse memory performance in AG can be partially explained by an IQ lower than the IQs in the comparison groups.

Figure 2
figure 2

Self-reference effect in memory (SRE) data. This figure plots the bootstrap distributions of the mean along with the 95% bias-corrected and accelerated bootstrap confidence intervals (vertical dotted lines). The ASC group is depicted in red and the control group in blue. AG is depicted as the black dotted line. Abbreviations: d′, memory sensitivity; ASC, autism spectrum condition; AgCC, agenesis of the corpus callosum.

Table 2 Self-reference effect in memory (SRE) paradigm a

Next we computed measures of the self-reference effect in memory, which is operationalized as the difference-score between memory sensitivity for self- and other non-self-encoding conditions (see Figure 3) [35, 83]. On this measure, an increasingly positive value means larger cognitive benefits for self-referential information processing. Given the descriptive statistics in Table 1, it is not surprising that we found that AgCC had an enhanced SRE difference-score compared to both the ASC and control groups for Self-Friend. However, this difference is driven primarily by the large drop-off in memory sensitivity in AG for the Friend and Potter conditions (relative to what is observed in the control and ASC groups). Given that the ASC and AgCC groups are predicted to have social-cognitive deficits, the social-cognitive conditions are not the best for using in such difference-scores as an assessment of whether one benefits from self-referential information processing. The reason here is that deficits in the social-cognitive domain can affect the difference-score, and, rather than it saying something about self-referential processing, it has more to say about social-cognitive deficits. Therefore, the Self-Friend and Self-Potter difference-scores do not provide an unambiguous measure of whether AG benefits from self-referential information processing.

Figure 3
figure 3

Self-reference effect in memory difference-score measures. This figure plots the bootstrap distributions of the mean difference-score of memory sensitivity measures on the SRE paradigm along with the 95% bias-corrected and accelerated bootstrap confidence intervals (vertical dotted lines). The top panel represents the self-reference effect in memory when Self-memory sensitivity is compared to Friend-memory sensitivity. The middle panel represents the self-reference effect in memory when Self-memory sensitivity is compared to Potter-memory sensitivity. The bottom panel represents the self-reference effect in memory when Self-memory sensitivity is compared to Syllable-memory sensitivity. The ASC group is depicted in red and the control group in blue. AG is depicted as the black dotted line. Abbreviations: d′, memory sensitivity; ASC, autism spectrum condition; AgCC, agenesis of the corpus callosum.

In contrast, the interpretation behind the Self-Syllable measure is different. Here the difference-score is not contaminated with effects due to both self-referential and social-cognitive processing and allows for a more unambiguous look at whether AG benefits from self-referential processing. AG shows relatively similar memory sensitivity for the Syllable-condition (see Table 1 and Figure 2) but more pronounced dropoff in the Self-condition, particularly with respect to the control group. Therefore, the Self-Syllable difference-score shows a smaller self-reference effect in memory for AG compared to the control group, but similar effect compared to the ASC group. This effect can be more unambiguously interpreted as a deficit for AG in cognitive benefits obtained from self-referential information processing because it is primarily driven by a dropoff in AG’s memory sensitivity for the Self-condition. This deficit is one that AG shares with the ASC group, but he cannot be considered an extreme of the ASC group.

On additional self-referential measures, AG’s performance was also significantly worse on the TAS and all its subscales and reported less private self-consciousness on the PSCS. However, on the SFA measure, AG used significantly more first-person pronouns than the ASC group but was similar to the control group. Thus, his performance on this test is not in keeping with the slightly lower production of first-person pronouns observed in the ASC group. See Figure 4 and Table 3. Correlational analysis showed that IQ was not significantly correlated with scores on these additional self-referential measures (see Additional file 1: Figure S2). This suggests that extreme scores observed in AG cannot be explained simply by lower IQ in AG.

Figure 4
figure 4

Additional self-referential measures. This figure plots the bootstrap distributions of the mean along with the 95% bias-corrected and accelerated bootstrap confidence intervals (vertical dotted lines). The ASC group is depicted in red and the control group in blue. AG is depicted as the black dotted line. Abbreviations: PSCS, Private Self-Consciousness Scale; SFA, self-focused attention index measured as the percentage of first person pronouns used in the Self-Focus Sentence Completion Task; TAS, Toronto Alexithymia Scale; DIF, Difficulty Identifying Feelings subscale of the TAS-20; DDF, Difficulty Describing Feelings subscale of the TAS-20; EOT, Externally Oriented Thinking subscale from the TAS-20; ASC, autism spectrum condition; AgCC, agenesis of the corpus callosum.

Table 3 Additional self-referential measures a

On additional social-cognitive measures, AG endorsed lower levels of empathy on the EQ and lower scores on the IRI perspective-taking and fantasy subscales compared to Control and ASC groups. AG also performed more poorly on the RMET task than both groups. In contrast, on the ECS and IRI-EC, AG self-reported significantly higher scores than the ASC group, but was within the confidence intervals for the control group. For IRI-PD, AG’s score was within the confidence intervals for the ASC group. See Figures 5 and 6 and Table 4. Correlational analysis showed that VIQ was not significantly correlated with any of these additional social-cognitive measures except the RMET. On the RMET, VIQ positively correlated with performance (r = 0.5988, P = 0.001) (see Additional file 1: Figure S2). This suggests that extreme performance on the RMET by AG could be partially explained by lower IQ. However, IQ could not explain extreme scores observed on other measures.

Figure 5
figure 5

Additional social-cognitive measures. This figure plots the bootstrap distributions of the mean along with the 95% bias-corrected and accelerated bootstrap confidence intervals (vertical dotted lines). The ASC group is depicted in red and the control group in blue. AG is depicted as the black dotted line. Abbreviations: IRI-EC, Empathic Concern subscale from the IRI; IRI-PT, Perspective Taking subscale of the IRI; IRI-FS, Fantasy subscale of the IRI; IRI-PD, Personal Distress subscale of the IRI; EQ, Empathy Quotient; ECS, Emotional Contagion Scale; ASC, autism spectrum condition; AgCC, agenesis of the corpus callosum.

Figure 6
figure 6

Reading the mind in the eyes test (RMET) performance. This figure plots the bootstrap distribution of the mean on the Reading the Mind in the Eyes test (RMET) along with the 95% bias-corrected and accelerated bootstrap confidence intervals (vertical dotted lines). The ASC group is depicted in red and the control group in blue. AG is depicted as the black dotted line. Abbreviations: RMET, Reading the Mind in the Eyes test; ASC, autism spectrum condition; AgCC, agenesis of the corpus callosum.

Table 4 Additional social-cognitive measures a

Discussion

In this study, we examined a patient with diagnoses of both ASC and AgCC on a range of self-referential and social-cognitive measures known to be atypical in ASC. As a potential neuroanatomical model for ASC, we followed the idea that AgCC might be a potential extreme of smaller CC volume consistently observed in ASC [12]. Thus, within social-cognitive and self-referential domains where there is some prior work showing potential abnormalities in both ASC and AgCC [10, 35, 48, 5254, 66, 70], we tested the idea that if the CC is critical in these domains, this patient with AgCC and ASC would show more extreme performance than that observed in both the ASC and control comparison groups. We found that across most measures in both domains, this patient with AgCC and ASC was atypical, even with respect to the ASC comparison group, in that deficits were more pronounced than what is typically observed on average in ASC.

Within the SRE paradigm, the ASC comparison group showed reduced memory sensitivity compared to controls specifically in the Self and Friend conditions [35]. In contrast, AG exhibited worse memory sensitivity compared to both comparison groups across all conditions. This pattern is partially suggestive of a general memory deficit that occurs across all levels of processing, from very deep encoding during self-referential processing to shallow encoding of information based on linguistic characteristics (i.e. counting syllables). However, the biggest differences were observed in social-cognitive conditions where memory was examined in relation to other people (i.e. Friend and Potter). This potentially suggests that while memory performance is generally affected, the areas that are hit hardest are social-cognitive information processing. Alternatively, part of the general trend for reduced memory across all conditions may be explained by AG’s lower IQ. VIQ was positively correlated with memory sensitivity in the ASC group for both Friend and Potter conditions. Given that AG also had a diagnosis of ASC, it is reasonable to infer from this result that a partial explanation for the reduced memory in these conditions could be because of lower IQ. However, it is also possible that IQ is not the only reason for decreased memory performance. Some variability in AG’s decreased performance is likely to be due to true effects in the domains of self-referential and social cognition. This is suggested by the lack of correlations with IQ observed on other self-referential and social-cognitive measures where AG also shows more extreme scores.

We also evaluated whether AG reaps cognitive benefits from self-referential information processing by comparing self-memory sensitivity to memory sensitivity across non-self conditions. Here we computed the difference-score between self-memory sensitivity and non-self-memory sensitivity, and increasingly positive scores indicate more benefit for self-referential information processing. Since we are dealing with groups that also have deficits in the social domain, using social-cognitive conditions such as Friend and Potter as the comparison to self-memory sensitivity is not ideal, because pronounced social-cognitive deficits can affect this memory sensitivity difference-score. This can clearly be seen in the difference-scores for Self-Friend and Self-Potter. Here AG appears to benefit substantially from self-referential information processing. However, this interpretation should be avoided, as the effects are driven primarily by the substantial drop-off in memory sensitivity for the Friend and Potter conditions in AG. Given these arguments, the most unambiguous measure for assessing whether AG reaps cognitive benefits of self-referential information processing is the Self-Syllable difference-score. Here memory sensitivity in the Syllable condition shows relatively less drop-off, and the difference-score cannot be confounded by additional deficits in social-cognitive processing. Here we find that AG benefits less from self-referential processing, as indexed by a lower Self-Syllable memory sensitivity difference-score compared to the control group. AG cannot be considered as an extreme from the ASC group though, because this Self-Syllable memory sensitivity difference-score was similar to what was observed in the ASC group.

Similar patterns of more extreme scores from AG were apparent in the self-referential domain on the PSCS, TAS, and subscales of the TAS and in the social-cognitive domain on the IRI-PT, IRI-FS, RMET, and EQ. Of these measures, only RMET correlated with IQ, suggesting that the extreme profile of scores from AG is relatively independent of his lower IQ. For the RMET, it may be that once IQ effects are accounted for, AG’s performance may not necessarily be at the extreme of the performance of the ASC group. In prior work, it is noteworthy that, in a larger sample of individuals with AgCC, there was overlap between the AgCC scores with both the control and ASC distributions on the RMET and no evidence of extreme deficits compared to the ASC group was observed [25]. However, the overall conjunction of extreme scores observed on both the SRE paradigm and across various other self-referential and social-cognitive measures suggests that AG’s performance represents an extreme with respect to what is observed on average in ASC. Thus, rather than AG exhibiting performance typical of ASC without AgCC, the current set of results is compatible with the idea that additional abnormalities in the CC in ASC may be relevant for understanding degree of self-referential and social-cognitive difficulties in ASC. To this end, focusing on cases with AgCC and a diagnosis of ASC may be a good neuroanatomical model for parsing the critical role that normative callosal development may have on these domains.

It should also be noted where AG was not an extreme with respect to the comparison groups. We found that on IRI-EC, IRI-PD, and ECS that AG’s scores were more similar to the control group. All of these measures share one commonality in being affective in nature. This suggests that the CC may not be as critical for understanding aspects of empathy difficulties in ASC that are affective in nature. This result stands in contrast to subscales measuring more cognitive aspects of empathy (i.e. perspective taking and fantasy subscales of the IRI), where AG was more extreme than both comparison groups. This may be suggestive that AgCC affects high-level cognitive components of social cognition while leaving affective components relatively spared. Within the self-referential domain, AG was also similar to the control group on the implicit measure of SFA that measures first person pronoun usage under conditions where the participant is primed to use self-references. While AG’s scores were significantly different from that observed in the ASC comparison group, the directionality is in keeping with a prior report showing that first person pronoun usage in narratives was increased in AgCC [46].

This apparent preservation of function in specific domains may be suggestive of two possibilities. First, there may be a certain degree of redundancy built into the neural architecture for affective function and pronoun usage, such that lacking large parts of anterior callosal regions do not have a major impact on these domains. Second, the developmental nature of AgCC might allow for appropriate compensatory mechanisms to develop over time in these domains. Future research could compare these results with that from patients who have an acquired white matter lesion in the anterior callosal region later in life. Finally, with respect to the self-reported affective empathy measures that appear to be spared, it remains to be seen whether AgCC patients would show similar spared function in the domain of affective empathy on performance-based tests. It may be that affective empathy is affected in AgCC, but that these patient’s self-referential difficulties obscure accurate measurements of their own affective empathy abilities.

There are several caveats and limitations to highlight about the current study. First, analyses showed that on some measures, variation related to IQ may be important. Given that our case (AG) had IQ scores that were lower than both comparison groups, it remains to be seen how well the results here are generalizable in studies where AgCC and comparison groups are matched on IQ. Our suggestion is that the role of IQ may be limited for interpreting the results here, as we found many pieces of evidence where AG’s performance was more extreme than the comparison groups and IQ was not generally correlated with performance. Second, this case report may not be fully generalizable to all patients with concurrent AgCC and ASC. It will be important for future work to expand on these observations with larger prospective work. Third, the current results are restricted to self-report measures and constrained experimental tasks involving situations likely not present during everyday functioning and real-world scenarios. Therefore, it remains to be seen whether the inferences from this study would bear out in future work using more naturalistic measures in real-world social situations. Fourth, the current work assesses the question about corpus callosum variation and autism from a categorical point of view. For example, in this study groups are defined categorically by their clinical diagnosis and within autism, the patient stands out as categorically distinct at the level of the corpus callosum. This is not the only way to investigate this question. Another way to address these types of questions is to look at the phenomenon from a continuous perspective where both autism and the corpus callosum are measured on a spectrum. This approach is intriguing and should be followed up in future work. This work sets up the hypothesis that from a dimensional perspective, better performance in areas of self-referential and social cognition might be related to continuous variation in CC size.

Finally, it is important to note the nature of AG’s callosal development. Rather than lacking a CC altogether, AG did have intact connections within the posterior section of the CC. At present there is no clear indication of whether partial versus complete AgCC cases are different with respect to performance in self-referential and social-cognitive domains. A recent MEG study found no differences between partial and complete AgCC with respect to global connectivity across alpha, gamma and beta bands [100]. Another recent study of fMRI resting state connectivity found strikingly similarities between AgCC and those with an intact CC. The exceptions to this were reduced connectivity in the posterior midline areas typically connected via the posterior sections of the CC [101]. Thus, because it has been postulated that these posterior midline areas may be important for self-referential and social-cognitive development [102], it is possible that AG’s partial connections make him a less extreme case than would be seen in complete AgCC. However, this remains open for future research on the topic.

Conclusions

In conclusion, we report that a case of partial AgCC and ASC showed more extreme scores than those typically found on measures that elicit group-differences in self-referential and social cognition in ASC compared to neurotypical controls. These results confirm prior findings suggesting that individuals with AgCC have marked problems in these domains. Furthermore, the current work suggests potential in viewing AgCC as a neuroanatomical model associated with ASC that may allow us to gain further insight into the mechanisms that may be important for understanding difficulties in self-referential and social cognition.

The MRC AIMS Consortium is a UK collaboration between the Institute of Psychiatry at Kings College, London, the Autism Research Centre and Brain Mapping Unit at the University of Cambridge, and the Autism Research Group at the University of Oxford. The Consortium members are, in alphabetical order, Bailey AJ, Baron-Cohen S, Bolton PF, Bullmore ET, Carrington S, Chakrabarti B, Daly EM, Deoni SC, Ecker C, Happé F, Henty J, Jezzard P, Johnston P, Jones DK, Lai, MC, Lombardo MV, Madden A, Mullins D, Murphy C, Murphy DG, Pasco G, Sadek SA, Spain D, Stewart R, Suckling J, Wheelwright S, Williams SC.

Abbreviations

ADI–R:

Autism Diagnostic Interview–Revised

ADOS:

Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule

AgCC:

Agenesis of the corpus callosum

AG:

Patient with partial agenesis of the corpus callosum and a clinical diagnosis of Asperger syndrome

AQ:

Autism spectrum quotient

AS:

Asperger syndrome

ASC:

Autism spectrum condition

CC:

Corpus callosum

d′:

Index of memory sensitivity

DDF:

Difficulty Describing Feelings subscale of the TAS

DIF:

Difficulty Indentifying Feelings subscale of the TAS

EOT:

Externally Oriented Thinking subscale of the TAS

EQ:

Empathy quotient

HFA:

High-functioning autism

IRI:

Interpersonal Reactivity Index

IRI-PT:

Perspective Taking subscale of the IRI

IRI-EC:

Empathic Concern subscale of the IRI

IRI-PD:

Personal Distress subscale of the IRI

IRI-FS:

Fantasy subscale of the IRI

ToM:

Theory of mind

MRI:

Magnetic resonance imaging

PSCS:

Private Self-Consciousness Scale

RMET:

Reading the Mind in the Eyes test

SFA:

Implicit measure of self-focused attention index by the percentage of first-person pronouns used in the Self-Focus Sentence Completion test

SRE:

Self-reference effect in memory paradigm

TAS:

Toronto Alexithymia Scale

VIQ:

Verbal IQ

WASI:

Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence.

References

  1. Kelleher RJ, Bear MF: The autistic neuron: troubled translation?. Cell. 2008, 135: 401-406. 10.1016/j.cell.2008.10.017.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Sanders SJ, Murtha MT, Gupta AR, Murdoch JD, Raubeson MJ, Willsey AJ, Ercan-Sencicek AG, DiLullo NM, Parikshak NN, Stein JL, Walker MF, Ober GT, Teran NA, Song Y, El-Fishawy P, Murtha RC, Choi M, Overton JD, Bjornson RD, Carriero NJ, Meyer KA, Bilguvar K, Mane SM, Sestan N, Lifton RP, Günel M, Roeder K, Geschwind DH, Devlin B, State MW: De novo mutations revealed by whole-exome sequencing are strongly associated with autism. Nature. 2012, 485: 237-241. 10.1038/nature10945.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Neale BM, Kou Y, Liu L, Ma’ayan A, Samocha KE, Sabo A, Lin CF, Stevens C, Wang LS, Makarov V, Polak P, Yoon S, Maguire J, Crawford EL, Campbell NG, Geller ET, Valladares O, Schafer C, Liu H, Zhao T, Cai G, Lihm J, Dannenfelser R, Jabado O, Peralta Z, Nagaswamy U, Muzny D, Reid JG, Newsham I, Wu Y: Patterns and rates of exonic de novo mutations in autism spectrum disorders. Nature. 2012, 485: 242-245. 10.1038/nature11011.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. O’Roak BJ, Vives L, Girirajan S, Karakoc E, Krumm N, Coe BP, Levy R, Ko A, Lee C, Smith JD, Turner EH, Stanaway IB, Vernot B, Malig M, Baker C, Reilly B, Akey JM, Borenstein E, Rieder MJ, Nickerson DA, Bernier R, Shendure J, Eichler EE: Sporadic autism exomes reveal a highly interconnected protein network of de novo mutations. Nature. 2012, 485: 246-250. 10.1038/nature10989.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  5. Auerbach BD, Osterweil EK, Bear MF: Mutations causing syndromic autism define an axis of synaptic pathophysiology. Nature. 2011, 480: 63-68. 10.1038/nature10658.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Konopka G, Wexler E, Rosen E, Mukamel Z, Osborn GE, Chen L, Lu D, Gao F, Gao K, Lowe JK, Geschwind DH: Modeling the functional genomics of autism using human neurons. Mol Psychiatry. 2012, 17: 202-214. 10.1038/mp.2011.60.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Marchetto MC, Carromeu C, Acab A, Yu D, Yeo GW, Mu Y, Chen G, Gage FH, Muotri AR: A model for neural development and treatment of Rett syndrome using human induced pluripotent stem cells. Cell. 2010, 143: 527-539. 10.1016/j.cell.2010.10.016.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Paşca SP, Portmann T, Voineagu I, Yazawa M, Shcheglovitov A, Paşca AM, Cord B, Palmer TD, Chikahisa S, Nishino S, Bernstein JA, Hallmayer J, Geschwind DH, Dolmetsch RE: Using iPSC-derived neurons to uncover cellular phenotypes associated with Timothy syndrome. Nat Med. 2011, 17: 1657-1662.9. 10.1038/nm.2576.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  9. Paul LK: Developmental malformation of the corpus callosum: a review of typical callosal development and examples of developmental disorders with callosal involvement. J Neurodev Disord. 2011, 3: 3-27. 10.1007/s11689-010-9059-y.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Paul LK, Brown WS, Adolphs R, Tyszka JM, Richards LJ, Mukherjee P, Sherr EH: Agenesis of the corpus callosum: genetic, developmental and functional aspects of connectivity. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2007, 8: 287-299. 10.1038/nrn2107.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Sherr EH: The ARX story (epilepsy, mental retardation, autism, and cerebral malformations): one gene leads to many phenotypes. Curr Opin Pediatr. 2003, 15: 567-571. 10.1097/00008480-200312000-00004.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Frazier TW, Hardan AY: A meta-analysis of the corpus callosum in autism. Biol Psychiatry. 2009, 66: 935-941. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.07.022.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Frazier TW, Keshavan MS, Minshew NJ, Hardan AY: A two-year longitudinal MRI study of the corpus callosum in autism. J Autism Dev Disord. 2012, 42: 2312-2322. 10.1007/s10803-012-1478-z.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Alexander AL, Lee JE, Lazar M, Boudos R, DuBray MB, Oakes TR, Miller JN, Lu J, Jeong EK, McMahon WM, Bigler ED, Lainhart JE: Diffusion tensor imaging of the corpus callosum in autism. Neuroimage. 2007, 34: 61-73.15. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.08.032.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Barnea-Goraly N, Kwon H, Menon V, Eliez S, Lotspeich L, Reiss AL: White matter structure in autism: preliminary evidence from diffusion tensor imaging. Biol Psychiatry. 2004, 55: 323-326. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2003.10.022.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Hong S, Ke X, Tang T, Hang Y, Chu K, Huang H, Ruan Z, Lu Z, Tao G, Liu Y: Detecting abnormalities of corpus callosum connectivity in autism using magnetic resonance imaging and diffusion tensor tractography. Psychiatry Res. 2011, 194: 333-339. 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2011.03.009.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Keller TA, Kana RK, Just MA: A developmental study of the structural integrity of white matter in autism. Neuroreport. 2007, 18: 23-27. 10.1097/01.wnr.0000239965.21685.99.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Kumar A, Sundaram SK, Sivaswamy L, Behen ME, Makki MI, Ager J, Janisse J, Chugani HT, Chugani DC: Alterations in frontal lobe tracts and corpus callosum in young children with autism spectrum disorder. Cereb Cortex. 2010, 20: 2103-2113. 10.1093/cercor/bhp278.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Shukla DK, Keehn B, Lincoln AJ, Müller RA: White matter compromise of callosal and subcortical fiber tracts in children with autism spectrum disorder: a diffusion tensor imaging study. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2010, 49: 1269-1278. e1–e2

    PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Bloemen OJ, Deeley Q, Sundram F, Daly EM, Barker GJ, Jones DK, van Amelsvoort TA, Schmitz N, Robertson D, Murphy KC, Murphy DG: White matter integrity in Asperger syndrome: a preliminary diffusion tensor magnetic resonance imaging study in adults. Autism Res. 2010, 3: 203-213. 10.1002/aur.146.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Gozzi M, Nielson DM, Lenroot RK, Ostuni JL, Luckenbaugh DA, Thurm AE, Giedd JN, Swedo SE: A magnetization transfer imaging study of corpus callosum myelination in young children with autism. Biol Psychiatry. 2012, 72: 215-220. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.01.026.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Bodensteiner J, Schaefer GB, Breeding L, Cowan L: Hypoplasia of the corpus callosum: a study of 445 consecutive MRI scans. J Child Neurol. 1994, 9: 47-49. 10.1177/088307389400900111.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Jeret JS, Serur D, Wisniewski K, Fisch C: Frequency of agenesis of the corpus callosum in the developmentally disabled population as determined by computerized tomography. Pediatr Neurosci. 1985, 12: 101-103. 10.1159/000120229.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Doherty D, Tu S, Schilmoeller K, Schilmoeller G: Health-related issues in individuals with agenesis of the corpus callosum. Child Care Health Dev. 2006, 32: 333-342. 10.1111/j.1365-2214.2006.00602.x.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Booth R, Wallace GL, Happé F: Connectivity and the corpus callosum in autism spectrum conditions: insights from comparison of autism and callosal agenesis. Prog Brain Res. 2011, 189: 303-317.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Badaruddin DH, Andrews GL, Bölte S, Schilmoeller KJ, Schilmoeller G, Paul LK, Brown WS: Social and behavioral problems of children with agenesis of the corpus callosum. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev. 2007, 38: 287-302. 10.1007/s10578-007-0065-6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Lau YC, Hinkley LB, Bukshpun P, Strominger ZA, Wakahiro ML, Baron-Cohen S, Allison C, Auyeung B, Jeremy RJ, Nagarajan SS, Sherr EH, Marco EJ: Autism traits in individuals with agenesis of the corpus callosum. J Autism Dev Disord. in press

  28. Baron-Cohen S: Mindblindness: An Essay on Autism and Theory of Mind. 1995, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press

    Google Scholar 

  29. Baron-Cohen S, Leslie AM, Frith U: Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind”?. Cognition. 1985, 21: 37-46. 10.1016/0010-0277(85)90022-8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Baron-Cohen S: Social and pragmatic deficits in autism: cognitive or affective?. J Autism Dev Disord. 1988, 18: 379-402. 10.1007/BF02212194.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Schwenck C, Mergenthaler J, Keller K, Zech J, Salehi S, Taurines R, Romanos M, Schecklmann M, Schneider W, Warnke A, Freitag CM: Empathy in children with autism and conduct disorder: group-specific profiles and developmental aspects. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2012, 53: 651-659. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2011.02499.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S, Hill J, Raste Y, Plumb I: The “Reading the Mind in the Eyes” Test revised version: a study with normal adults, and adults with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2001, 42: 241-251. 10.1111/1469-7610.00715.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Jones AP, Happé FG, Gilbert F, Burnett S, Viding E: Feeling, caring, knowing: different types of empathy deficit in boys with psychopathic tendencies and autism spectrum disorder. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2010, 51: 1188-1197. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02280.x.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S: The empathy quotient: an investigation of adults with Asperger syndrome or high functioning autism, and normal sex differences. J Autism Dev Disord. 2004, 34: 163-175.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Lombardo MV, Barnes JL, Wheelwright SJ, Baron-Cohen S: Self-referential cognition and empathy in autism. PLoS One. 2007, 2: e883-10.1371/journal.pone.0000883.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Rogers K, Dziobek I, Hassenstab J, Wolf OT, Convit A: Who cares? Revisiting empathy in Asperger syndrome. J Autism Dev Disord. 2007, 37: 709-715. 10.1007/s10803-006-0197-8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Lai MC, Lombardo MV, Ruigrok ANV, Chakrabarti B, Wheelwright SJ, Auyeung B, Allison C, Baron-Cohen S, MRC AIMS Consortium: Cognition in males and females with autism: similarities and differences. PLoS One. 2012, 7: e47198-10.1371/journal.pone.0047198.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Silani G, Bird G, Brindley R, Singer T, Frith C, Frith U: Levels of emotional awareness and autism: an fMRI study. Soc Neurosci. 2008, 3: 97-110. 10.1080/17470910701577020.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Minio-Paluello I, Baron-Cohen S, Avenanti A, Walsh V, Aglioti SM: Absence of embodied empathy during pain observation in Asperger syndrome. Biol Psychiatry. 2009, 65: 55-62. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.08.006.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Minio-Paluello I, Lombardo MV, Chakrabarti B, Wheelwright S, Baron-Cohen S: Response to Smith’s letter to the editor: ‘Emotional Empathy in Autism Spectrum Conditions: Weak, Intact, or Heightened?’. J Autism Dev Disord. 2009, 39: 1749-1754. 10.1007/s10803-009-0800-x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. Brown WS, Symingtion M, VanLancker-Sidtis D, Dietrich R, Paul LK: Paralinguistic processing in children with callosal agenesis: emergence of neurolinguistic deficits. Brain Lang. 2005, 93: 135-139. 10.1016/j.bandl.2004.09.003.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Paul LK, Van Lancker-Sidtis D, Schieffer B, Dietrich R, Brown WS: Communicative deficits in agenesis of the corpus callosum: nonliteral language and affective prosody. Brain Lang. 2003, 85: 313-324. 10.1016/S0093-934X(03)00062-2.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Brown WS, Paul LK, Symington M, Dietrich R: Comprehension of humor in primary agenesis of the corpus callosum. Neuropsychologia. 2005, 43: 906-916. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.09.008.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Huber-Okrainec J, Blaser SE, Dennis M: Idiom comprehension deficits in relation to corpus callosum agenesis and hypoplasia in children with spina bifida meningomyelocele. Brain Lang. 2005, 93: 349-368. 10.1016/j.bandl.2004.11.002.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Paul LK, Schieffer B, Brown WS: Social processing deficits in agenesis of the corpus callosum: narratives from the Thematic Appreciation Test. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2004, 19: 215-225. 10.1016/S0887-6177(03)00024-6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Turk AA, Brown WS, Symington M, Paul LK: Social narratives in agenesis of the corpus callosum: linguistic analysis of the Thematic Apperception Test. Neuropsychologia. 2010, 48: 43-50. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.08.009.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Barnes JL, Lombardo MV, Wheelwright S, Baron-Cohen S: Moral dilemmas film task: a study of spontaneous narratives by individuals with autism spectrum conditions. Autism Res. 2009, 2: 148-156. 10.1002/aur.79.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Symington SH, Paul LK, Symington MF, Ono M, Brown WS: Social cognition in individuals with agenesis of the corpus callosum. Soc Neurosci. 2010, 5: 296-308. 10.1080/17470910903462419.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Happé FG: An advanced test of theory of mind: understanding of story characters’ thoughts and feelings in able autistic, mentally handicapped, and normal children and adults. J Autism Dev Disord. 1994, 24: 129-154. 10.1007/BF02172093.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Baron-Cohen S, O’Riordan M, Stone V, Jones R, Plaisted K: Recognition of faux pas by normally developing children and children with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. J Autism Dev Disord. 1999, 29: 407-418. 10.1023/A:1023035012436.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Paul LK, Lautzenhiser A, Brown WS, Hart A, Neumann D, Spezio M, Adolphs R: Emotional arousal in agenesis of the corpus callosum. Int J Psychophysiol. 2006, 61: 47-56. 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2005.10.017.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Lombardo MV, Baron-Cohen S: Unraveling the paradox of the autistic self. Wiley Interdiscip Rev Cogn Sci. 2010, 1: 393-403. 10.1002/wcs.45.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Lombardo MV, Baron-Cohen S: The role of the self in mindblindness in autism. Conscious Cogn. 2011, 20: 130-140. 10.1016/j.concog.2010.09.006.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. Lombardo MV, Chakrabarti B, Bullmore ET, Sadek SA, Wheelwright SJ, Pasco G, Suckling J, Baron-Cohen S, MRC AIMS Consortium: Atypical neural self-representation in autism. Brain. 2010, 133: 611-624. 10.1093/brain/awp306.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. Hill E, Berthoz S, Frith U: Brief report: cognitive processing of own emotions in individuals with autistic spectrum disorder and in their relatives. J Autism Dev Disord. 2004, 34: 229-235.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. Lind SE: Memory and the self in autism: a review and theoretical framework. Autism. 2010, 14: 430-456. 10.1177/1362361309358700.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Hurlburt RT, Happé F, Frith U: Sampling the form of inner experience in three adults with Asperger syndrome. Psychol Med. 1994, 24: 385-395. 10.1017/S0033291700027367.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  58. Hobson PR, Chidambi G, Lee A, Meyer J: Foundations for self-awareness: an exploration through autism. Monogr Soc Res Child Dev. 2006, 71: 1-166.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  59. Nadig AS, Ozonoff S, Young GS, Rozga A, Sigman M, Rogers SJ: A prospective study of response to name in infants at risk for autism. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2007, 161: 378-383. 10.1001/archpedi.161.4.378.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  60. Osterling J, Dawson G: Early recognition of children with autism: a study of first birthday home videotapes. J Autism Dev Disord. 1994, 24: 247-257. 10.1007/BF02172225.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  61. Phillips W, Baron-Cohen S, Rutter M: Understanding intention in normal development and in autism. Br J Dev Psychol. 1998, 16: 337-348. 10.1111/j.2044-835X.1998.tb00756.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  62. Williams DM, Happé F: What did I say? Versus what did I think? Attributing false beliefs to self amongst children with and without autism. J Autism Dev Disord. 2009, 39: 865-873. 10.1007/s10803-009-0695-6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  63. Mitchell P, O’Keefe K: Brief report: Do individuals with autism spectrum disorder think they know their own minds?. J Autism Dev Disord. 2008, 38: 1591-1597. 10.1007/s10803-007-0530-x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  64. Hobson RP: Explaining autism: ten reasons to focus on the developing self. Autism. 2010, 14: 391-407. 10.1177/1362361310364142.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  65. Williams D: Theory of own mind in autism: evidence of a specific deficit in self-awareness?. Autism. 2010, 14: 474-494. 10.1177/1362361310366314.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  66. Buchanan DC, Waterhouse GJ, West SC: A proposed neurophysiological basis of alexithymia. Psychother Psychosom. 1980, 34: 248-255. 10.1159/000287465.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  67. Zeitlin SB, Lane RD, O’Leary DS, Schrift MJ: Interhemispheric transfer deficit and alexithymia. Am J Psychiatry. 1989, 146: 1434-1439.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  68. TenHouten WD, Hoppe KD, Bogen JE, Walter DO: Alexithymia: an experimental study of cerebral commissurotomy patients and normal control subjects. Am J Psychiatry. 1986, 143: 312-316.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  69. Hoppe KD, Bogen JE: Alexithymia in twelve commissurotomized patients. Psychother Psychosom. 1977, 28: 148-155. 10.1159/000287057.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  70. Brown WS, Paul LK: Cognitive and psychosocial deficits in agenesis of the corpus callosum with normal intelligence. Cogn Neuropsychiatry. 2000, 5: 135-157. 10.1080/135468000395781.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  71. Ecker C, Suckling J, Deoni SC, Lombardo MV, Bullmore ET, Baron-Cohen S, Catani M, Jezzard P, Barnes A, Bailey AJ, Williams SC, Murphy DG, MRC AIMS Consortium: Brain anatomy and its relationship to behavior in adults with autism spectrum disorder: a multicenter magnetic resonance imaging study. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2012, 69: 195-209. 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.1251.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  72. Ecker C, Rocha-Rego V, Johnston P, Mourao-Miranda J, Marquand A, Daly EM, Brammer MJ, Murphy C, Murphy DG, MRC AIMS Consortium: Investigating the predictive value of whole-brain structural MR scans in autism: a pattern classification approach. Neuroimage. 2010, 49: 44-56. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.08.024.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  73. Lai MC, Lombardo MV, Chakrabarti B, Ecker C, Sadek SA, Wheelwright SJ, Murphy DG, Suckling J, Bullmore ET, Baron-Cohen S, MRC AIMS Consortium: Individual differences in brain structure underpin empathizing-systemizing cognitive styles in male adults. Neuroimage. 2012, 61: 1347-1354. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.03.018.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  74. Lai MC, Lombardo MV, Chakrabarti B, Sadek SA, Pasco G, Wheelwright SJ, Bullmore ET, Baron-Cohen S, Suckling J, MRC AIMS Consortium: A shift to randomness of brain oscillations in people with autism. Biol Psychiatry. 2010, 68: 1092-1099. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.06.027.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  75. Lai MC, Lombardo MV, Pasco G, Ruigrok AN, Wheelwright SJ, Sadek SA, Chakrabarti B, Baron-Cohen S: A behavioral comparison of male and female adults with high functioning autism spectrum conditions. PLoS One. 2011, 6: e20835-10.1371/journal.pone.0020835.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  76. Langen M, Leemans A, Johnston P, Ecker C, Daly E, Murphy CM, Dell’acqua F, Durston S, Murphy DG, MRC AIMS Consortium: Fronto-striatal circuitry and inhibitory control in autism: findings from diffusion tensor imaging tractography. Cortex. 2012, 48: 183-193. 10.1016/j.cortex.2011.05.018.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  77. Lombardo MV, Chakrabarti B, Bullmore ET, Baron-Cohen S, MRC AIMS Consortium: Specialization of right temporo-parietal junction for mentalizing and its association with social impairments in autism. Neuroimage. 2011, 56: 1832-1838. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.02.067.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  78. Lombardo MV, Chakrabarti B, Bullmore ET, Wheelwright SJ, Sadek SA, Suckling J, Baron-Cohen S, MRC AIMS Consortium: Shared neural circuits for mentalizing about the self and others. J Cogn Neurosci. 2010, 22: 1623-1635. 10.1162/jocn.2009.21287.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  79. Wahl M, Strominger Z, Jeremy RJ, Barkovich AJ, Wakahiro M, Sherr EH, Mukherjee P: Variability of homotopic and heterotopic callosal connectivity in partial agenesis of the corpus callosum: a 3T diffusion tensor imaging and Q-ball tractography study. Am J Neuroradiol. 2009, 30: 282-289.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  80. Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S, Skinner R, Martin J, Clubley E: The autism-spectrum quotient (AQ): evidence from Asperger syndrome/high-functioning autism, males and females, scientists and mathematicians. J Autism Dev Disord. 2001, 31: 5-17. 10.1023/A:1005653411471.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  81. Weschler D: Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence. 1999, San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corp

    Google Scholar 

  82. Craik FIM, Tulving E: Depth of processing and the retention of words in episodic memory. J Exp Psychol Gen. 1975, 11: 268-294.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  83. Symons CS, Johnson BT: The self-reference effect in memory: a meta-analysis. Psychol Bull. 1997, 121: 371-394.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  84. Klein SB, Kihlstrom JF: Elaboration, organization, and the self-reference effect in memory. J Exp Psychol Gen. 1986, 115: 26-38.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  85. Klein SB, Loftus J: The nature of self-referent encoding: the contributions of elaborative and organizational processes. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1988, 55: 5-11.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  86. Pfeifer JH, Lieberman MD, Dapretto M: “I know you are but what am I?”: Neural bases of self- and social knowledge retrieval in children and adults. J Cogn Neurosci. 2007, 19: 1323-1337. 10.1162/jocn.2007.19.8.1323.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  87. Anderson NH: Likableness ratings of 555 personality-trait words. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1968, 9: 272-279.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  88. Fenigstein A, Scheier MF, Buss AH: Public and private self-consciousness: assessment and theory. J Consult Clin Psychol. 1975, 43: 522-527.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  89. Bagby RM, Parker JD, Taylor GJ: The twenty-item Toronto Alexithymia Scale I. Item selection and cross-validation of the factor structure. J Psychosom Res. 1994, 38: 23-32. 10.1016/0022-3999(94)90005-1.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  90. Exner JE: The self-focus sentence completion: a study of egocentricity. J Pers Assess. 1973, 37: 437-455. 10.1080/00223891.1973.10119902.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  91. Pennebaker JW, Francis ME, Booth RJ: Linguistic Inquiry and Word Count (LIWC): A Computerized Text Analysis Program. 2001, Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum

    Google Scholar 

  92. Davis D, Brock TC: Use of first person pronouns as a function of increased objective self-awareness and prior feedback. J Exp Soc Psychol. 1975, 11: 381-388. 10.1016/0022-1031(75)90017-7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  93. Ingram RE, Cruet D, Johnson BR, Wisnicki KS: Self-focused attention, gender, gender role, and vulnerability to negative affect. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1988, 55: 967-978.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  94. Rude SS, Gortner E, Pennebaker JW: Language use of depressed and depression-vulnerable college students. Cogn Emotion. 2004, 18: 1121-1133. 10.1080/02699930441000030.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  95. Davis MH: A multidimensional approach to individual differences in empathy. JSAS Catalogue of Selected Documents in Psychology. 1980, 10: 85-

    Google Scholar 

  96. Doherty RW: The emotional contagion scale: a measure of individual differences. J Nonverb Behav. 1997, 21: 131-154. 10.1023/A:1024956003661.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  97. Efron B, Tibshirani RJ: An Introduction to the Bootstrap. 1993, New York: Chapman Hall

    Book  Google Scholar 

  98. Huber PJ: Robust Statistics. 1981, New York: Wiley-Interscience

    Book  Google Scholar 

  99. Marco EJ, Harrell KM, Brown WS, Hill SS, Jeremy RJ, Kramer JH, Sherr EH, Paul LK: Processing speed delays contribute to executive function deficits in individuals with agenesis of the corpus callosum. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2012, 18: 521-529. 10.1017/S1355617712000045.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  100. Hinkley LB, Marco EJ, Findlay AM, Honma S, Jeremy RJ, Strominger Z, Bukshpun P, Wakahiro M, Brown WS, Paul LK, Barkovich AJ, Mukherjee P, Nagarajan SS, Sherr EH: The role of corpus callosum development in functional connectivity and cognitive processing. PLoS One. 2012, 7: e39804-10.1371/journal.pone.0039804.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  101. Tyszka JM, Kennedy DP, Adolphs R, Paul LK: Intact bilateral resting-state networks in the absence of the corpus callosum. J Neurosci. 2011, 31: 15154-15162. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1453-11.2011.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  102. Pfeifer JH, Peake SJ: Self-development: integrating cognitive, socioemotional, and neuroimaging perspectives. Dev Cogn Neurosci. 2012, 2: 55-69. 10.1016/j.dcn.2011.07.012.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We acknowledge support from the Medical Research Council (MRC) Autism Imaging Multi-Centre Study (AIMS) Consortium. We thank Jennifer Barnes, Liliana Ruta, Greg Pasco, Sally Wheelwright, and Erin Ingudomnukul for help with data collection for the comparison groups. MVL was supported by the Shirley Foundation, the Cambridge Overseas Trust, Jesus College Cambridge, and the British Academy during this period of work. This work was conducted in association with the NIHR CLAHRC for Cambridgeshire and Peterborough NHS Foundation Trust. All funding bodies had no role in study design, data collection, analysis, and interpretation of the data. The MRC AIMS Consortium is a UK collaboration between the Institute of Psychiatry at Kings College, London, the Autism Research Centre and Brain Mapping Unit at the University of Cambridge, and the Autism Research Group at the University of Oxford. The Consortium members are, in alphabetical order, Bailey AJ, Baron-Cohen S, Bolton PF, Bullmore ET, Carrington S, Chakrabarti B, Daly EM, Deoni SC, Ecker C, Happé F, Henty J, Jezzard P, Johnston P, Jones DK, Lai, MC, Lombardo MV, Madden A, Mullins D, Murphy C, Murphy DG, Pasco G, Sadek SA, Spain D, Stewart R, Suckling J, Wheelwright S, Williams SC.

Financial disclosure

None of the authors have any biomedical financial interests or potential conflicts of interest.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Consortia

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Michael V Lombardo.

Additional information

Competing interest

The authors declare they have no competing interest.

Authors’ contributions

MVL conceived and carried out the study, ran all statistical analyses, and drafted the manuscript. BC, MCL, MRC AIMS Consortium, and SBC contributed to study design. BC, MCL, and SBC helped draft the manuscript. SBC and the MRC AIMS Consortium obtained funding and coordinated the study. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Electronic supplementary material

13229_2012_44_MOESM1_ESM.doc

Additional file 1: Figure S1. Reaction times on SRE paradigm. Figure S2. Correlations between IQ scales and dependent variables within the ASC group. (DOC 691 KB)

Authors’ original submitted files for images

Rights and permissions

This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd. This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Lombardo, M.V., Chakrabarti, B., Lai, MC. et al. Self-referential and social cognition in a case of autism and agenesis of the corpus callosum. Molecular Autism 3, 14 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1186/2040-2392-3-14

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/2040-2392-3-14

Keywords